1977
DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.3.1.27
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effects of context on the classification of words and nonwords.

Abstract: Observers were required to classify letter strings as words or nonwords under the following three conditions: (a) when the target stimulus alone was presented, (b) when the target was preceded by an incomplete sentence, and (c) when the target was preceded by a string of four spelled-out digits. Word targets were either high-or low-frequency items and either semantically congruous or incongruous with respect to the incomplete sentence. Nonword targets were either pronounceable or nonpronounceable. The presenta… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

11
128
3
1

Year Published

1979
1979
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
2

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 169 publications
(143 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
11
128
3
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This result is compatible with Schubert and Eimas' (1977) demonstration that context and frequency have additive effects in a lexical decision task. The observed stability of word-frequency effects across single word and text presentation is an important demonstration of the fundamental role played by word frequency in visual word recognition.…”
Section: Comments On Word-length and Word-frequency Effectssupporting
confidence: 80%
“…This result is compatible with Schubert and Eimas' (1977) demonstration that context and frequency have additive effects in a lexical decision task. The observed stability of word-frequency effects across single word and text presentation is an important demonstration of the fundamental role played by word frequency in visual word recognition.…”
Section: Comments On Word-length and Word-frequency Effectssupporting
confidence: 80%
“…Reaction times as well as eye movement data point to faster processing of high than of low predictability words (e.g., Ashby, Rayner, & Clifton, 2005;Calvo & Meseguer;Duffy, Henderson, & Morris, 1989;Ehrlich & Rayner, 1981;Fischler & Bloom, 1979;Kleiman, 1980;Kliegl et al, 2004Kliegl et al, , 2006Rayner, Ashby, Pollatsek, & Reichle, 2004;Rayner, Binder, Ashby, & Pollatsek, 2001;Rayner & Well, 1996;Schuberth & Eimas, 1977;Stanovich & West, 1983;West & Stanovich, 1982). The critical question, however, is when does top-down expectation of a stimulus interact with the incoming visual information during reading and, depending on whether the prediction was correct or wrong, when does it help or hurt word processing?…”
Section: Predictability: Top-down Expectationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The effect of the semantic relatedness of the context and the stimulus on decision latency has been found (1) to interact with the effect of the visual quality (Meyer et al, 1975) and intensity (Becker & Killion, 1977) of the stimulus and (2) to combine additively with the counted frequency of occurrence of the stimulus in the English language (Schuberth & Eimas, 1977; but see also Becker, 1979). In addition, the effect of word frequency on decision latency has been shown to combine additively with the effects of the visual quality (Stanners, Jastrzembski, & Westbrook, 1975) and intensity (Becker & Killion, 1977) of the stimulus.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…whether single words (Becker, 1980;Neely, 1976Neely, , 1977 or incomplete sentences (Fischler & Bloom, 1979Schuberth & Eimas, 1977;Stanovich, 1980) were used as context.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%