2014
DOI: 10.1007/s10902-014-9596-2
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effectiveness of Two Cognitive Interventions Promoting Happiness with Video-Based Online Instructions

Abstract: A 3-month experimental online study examined the short-term and 1 month follow-up effects of regularly practicing one of two cognitive interventions on subjective well-being. Participants were 435 self-selected adults (366 female, 69 male, aged 18-63) randomly assigned to one of three conditions: writing about best possible selves in the future (n = 135), making gratitude lists (n = 150) or writing to-do-lists as a control condition (n = 150). The study was fully self-administered and exercise instructions wer… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

2
38
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
3

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 48 publications
(42 citation statements)
references
References 66 publications
(82 reference statements)
2
38
0
Order By: Relevance
“…A few studies report evidence for physiological resource gains (e.g., Bono et al, ; Jackowska, Brown, Ronaldson, & Steptoe, ), but the vast majority examine a narrow set of psychological resources, with the most common being happiness (e.g., Mongrain & Anselmo‐Matthews, ; Seligman et al, ), life satisfaction (e.g., Manthey, Vehreschild, & Renner, ; Rust et al, ), positive mood or affect (e.g., Emmons & McCullough, ; Sheldon & Lyubomirsky, ), and optimism (e.g., Meevissen, Peters, & Alberts, ). Several meta‐analyses have reported on the average effect sizes across intervention types: Sin and Lyubomirsky () reported an average effect of r = .29 for well‐being, whereas Bolier et al (), using stricter inclusion criteria and only randomized controlled trials, reported somewhat smaller standardized mean differences: d = 0.34 for subjective well‐being (e.g., life satisfaction and positive affect) and d = 0.20 for psychological well‐being (positive psychological functioning) at posttest.…”
Section: Interventionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…A few studies report evidence for physiological resource gains (e.g., Bono et al, ; Jackowska, Brown, Ronaldson, & Steptoe, ), but the vast majority examine a narrow set of psychological resources, with the most common being happiness (e.g., Mongrain & Anselmo‐Matthews, ; Seligman et al, ), life satisfaction (e.g., Manthey, Vehreschild, & Renner, ; Rust et al, ), positive mood or affect (e.g., Emmons & McCullough, ; Sheldon & Lyubomirsky, ), and optimism (e.g., Meevissen, Peters, & Alberts, ). Several meta‐analyses have reported on the average effect sizes across intervention types: Sin and Lyubomirsky () reported an average effect of r = .29 for well‐being, whereas Bolier et al (), using stricter inclusion criteria and only randomized controlled trials, reported somewhat smaller standardized mean differences: d = 0.34 for subjective well‐being (e.g., life satisfaction and positive affect) and d = 0.20 for psychological well‐being (positive psychological functioning) at posttest.…”
Section: Interventionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A few studies report evidence for physiological resource gains (e.g., Bono et al, 2013;Jackowska, Brown, Ronaldson, & Steptoe, 2015), but the vast majority examine a narrow set of psychological resources, with the most common being happiness (e.g., Mongrain & Anselmo-Matthews, 2012;Seligman et al, 2005), life satisfaction (e.g., Manthey, Vehreschild, & Renner, 2016;Rust et al, 2009), positive mood or affect (e.g., Emmons & McCullough, 2003;Sheldon & Lyubomirsky, 2006), and optimism (e.g., Meevissen, Peters, & Alberts, 2011). Several meta-analyses have reported on the average effect sizes across intervention types: Sin and Lyubomirsky (2009) (Bolier et al, 2013;Sin & Lyubomirsky, 2009) show that self-selected volunteers derived greater well-being benefits (average r = .35) than did participants who were assigned (average r = .20).…”
Section: Positive Psychology Interventionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In a randomized controlled trial using a cognitive-behavioral tool, Powell et al [ 30 ] showed improvement in well-being in a 12-week follow-up in the intervention group compared to controls. In a study with 435 self-selected adults, both writing about best possible selves and making gratitude lists improved subjective well-being compared to writing to-do-lists, and the effect was maintained in the one-month follow-up [ 31 ]. A study with a 6-month follow-up noticed that self-compassion and optimism exercises were able to increase happiness in persons vulnerable to depression [ 32 ].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Gratitude interventions often take the form of compiling a list of things to be grateful for in a journal (gratitude journals/lists; Wood et al, 2010), brief grateful contemplation (Watkins, Woodward, Stone, & Kolts, 2003), or writing letters of gratitude (Seligman, Steen, Park, & Peterson, 2005). Others, notably when contrasting gratitude with another positive psychology intervention (e.g., writing about best possible selves), have found little evidence that gratitude interventions were superior (Kerr, O'Donovan, & Pepping, 2015;Manthey, Vehreschild, & Renner, 2016), begging the question of whether gratitude interventions are distinct from other positive psychology interventions (Dickens, 2017). Some gratitude interventions or PPIs with a gratitude component have demonstrated improvements in psychological well-being (e.g., increases in positive affect, reductions in depression; Seligman et al, 2005;Watkins, Uhder, & Pichinevskiy, 2014;Wood et al, 2010) and, more recently, in improved biological functioning (e.g., blood pressure) in healthy populations (Jackowska, Brown, Ronaldson, & Steptoe, 2016).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some gratitude interventions or PPIs with a gratitude component have demonstrated improvements in psychological well-being (e.g., increases in positive affect, reductions in depression; Seligman et al, 2005;Watkins, Uhder, & Pichinevskiy, 2014;Wood et al, 2010) and, more recently, in improved biological functioning (e.g., blood pressure) in healthy populations (Jackowska, Brown, Ronaldson, & Steptoe, 2016). Others, notably when contrasting gratitude with another positive psychology intervention (e.g., writing about best possible selves), have found little evidence that gratitude interventions were superior (Kerr, O'Donovan, & Pepping, 2015;Manthey, Vehreschild, & Renner, 2016), begging the question of whether gratitude interventions are distinct from other positive psychology interventions (Dickens, 2017).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%