2003
DOI: 10.1152/jn.00656.2002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effect of Visual Error Size on Saccade Adaptation in Monkey

Abstract: Saccades that consistently over- or undershoot their targets gradually become smaller or larger, respectively. The signal that elicits adaptation of saccade size is a difference between eye and target positions appearing repeatedly at the ends of saccades. Here we describe how visual error size affects the size of saccade adaptation. At the end of each saccade, we imposed a constant-sized error by moving the target to a specified point relative to eye position. We tested a variety of error sizes imposed after … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

17
110
1
1

Year Published

2004
2004
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 112 publications
(129 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
17
110
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The inverse relationship between pitch error size and vocal learning reported here parallels similar findings in the response of the human arm movement system to visual (6) and force (3) perturbations and of the primate oculomotor system to visual errors during saccades (4). Similarly, in response to brief (200-500 ms) pitch perturbations during ongoing speech, transient compensatory vocal changes in human subjects occur on fewer trials (16) and correct a smaller fraction of the imposed error (17) for larger perturbations.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 86%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The inverse relationship between pitch error size and vocal learning reported here parallels similar findings in the response of the human arm movement system to visual (6) and force (3) perturbations and of the primate oculomotor system to visual errors during saccades (4). Similarly, in response to brief (200-500 ms) pitch perturbations during ongoing speech, transient compensatory vocal changes in human subjects occur on fewer trials (16) and correct a smaller fraction of the imposed error (17) for larger perturbations.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 86%
“…In contrast, some studies of arm and eye movements have shown that as the size of an experimentally induced sensory error increases, the size of the behavioral response approaches an asymptote (3) or begins to decline (4,6). Furthermore, when the magnitude of an auditory perturbation is gradually increased across a single training session, vocal compensation by human subjects can plateau or begin to decrease (7,8), suggesting that larger errors are less effective at driving learning.…”
mentioning
confidence: 97%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…6) would constantly signal whether saccades were staying accurate. Consistent with this suggestion, Robinson et al (2003) recently showed that smaller errors are more efficient at driving adaptation than are large ones. Finally, a model that uses an error signal that indicates only the direction of saccade dysmetria is sufficient to simulate the general properties of saccade adaptation (Dean et al, 1994).…”
Section: Do Changes In Cs Activity Drive Saccade Adaptation?mentioning
confidence: 58%
“…If the shift vector was place-coded in the SC, why did stimulation at sites representing errors Ͼ5°fail to elicit clear adaptation-like changes? Robinson et al (2003) examined the effect of error size on saccade adaptation using errors whose size was fixed during adaptation. They found that errors Ͻ45% the size of the target step elicited average gain changes of ϳ0.3-0.4, whereas error sizes Ͼ45% of the target step produced much lesser gain changes.…”
Section: Representation Of Instructions For Saccade Adaptation In the Scmentioning
confidence: 99%