1963
DOI: 10.1037/h0044357
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effect of verbal, visual, and auditory augmenting cues on learning a complex motor skill.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

1965
1965
1997
1997

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 21 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 9 publications
(10 reference statements)
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The usual finding is that people who have practiced with concurrent continuous feedback often perform worse on no-feedback retention tests than do people who have practiced without such feedback. In other words, continuous concurrent feedback appears to enhance performance during practice when the feedback is operating, but it does not contribute to learning and may even degrade learning, as measured on retention and transfer tests (see Annett, 1959Annett, , 1969Karlin & Mortimer, 1963;Kohl & Shea, 1995;Patrick & Mutlusoy, 1982;Van der Linden et aI., 1993).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 97%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The usual finding is that people who have practiced with concurrent continuous feedback often perform worse on no-feedback retention tests than do people who have practiced without such feedback. In other words, continuous concurrent feedback appears to enhance performance during practice when the feedback is operating, but it does not contribute to learning and may even degrade learning, as measured on retention and transfer tests (see Annett, 1959Annett, , 1969Karlin & Mortimer, 1963;Kohl & Shea, 1995;Patrick & Mutlusoy, 1982;Van der Linden et aI., 1993).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…This feedback can also be presented continuously (on-line, with essentially no delay) to indicate the level of momentary performance error (Karlin & Mortimer, 1963;Kohl & Shea, 1995;Phillips & Berkhout, 1978), deviations from a goal movement pattern (Van der Linden, Cauraugh, & Greene, 1993), the pattern of ongoing electromyographic activity (Mulder & Hulstijn, 1985), or other sources of biofeedback. Continuous concurrent augmented feedback using vision is the focus of the present article.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For simple tasks (defined as exact replications of a prespecified movement rather than achieving a desired effect on the environment), various forms of augmented feedback have been applied with mixed effects on performance. No advantage in skill acquisition has been found when auditory or visual cues are provided in addition to error information during visual tracking (Bilodeau & Rosenquist, 1964;Cote, Williges, & Williges, 1981;Karlin & Mortimer, 1963). Young and Schmidt (1992) demonstrated that in a task involving a swing of the forearm backward and then forward, augmented feedback on the reversal position enhanced performance (compared with KR alone), although information about the variability of the reversal position, or the time of reversal, had no effect.…”
mentioning
confidence: 95%
“…Whether the contradictory results of these studies derive from the use of tones vs. clicks or from population differences was not clear. Other investigators have either found slight decrements with more Feedback and Tracking Skill complex tracking tasks (Archer, Kent, & Mote, 1956) or significant increments (E. Bilodeau, 1969;Karlin & Mortimer, 1963;Kinkade, 1963;Smode, 1958;Williams & Briggs, 1962). Positive results were more common with such devices as the Pedestal Sight Manipulation Test (PSMT) and the Multidimensional Pursuit Test (MDPT).…”
Section: Clyde E Noble and Charles S Noblementioning
confidence: 92%