1967
DOI: 10.1177/001316446702700448
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effect of Perceived Scoring Formula On Some Aspects of Test Performance

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

1972
1972
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 4 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…There is, to the author's knowledge, no direct empirical evidence for this suggestion where CTD are concerned. However, if one extends the argument "by analogy" from the similar results obtained in the Sherriffs and Boomer (1954) and Hritz and Jacobs (1970) studies, the results of Waters (1967) may be relevant. Waters found that increased levels of penalty resulted in significant increases in the number of omitted items in a conventional multiple-choice testing situation.…”
mentioning
confidence: 75%
“…There is, to the author's knowledge, no direct empirical evidence for this suggestion where CTD are concerned. However, if one extends the argument "by analogy" from the similar results obtained in the Sherriffs and Boomer (1954) and Hritz and Jacobs (1970) studies, the results of Waters (1967) may be relevant. Waters found that increased levels of penalty resulted in significant increases in the number of omitted items in a conventional multiple-choice testing situation.…”
mentioning
confidence: 75%
“…The failure of examinee behavior to conform to the knowledge-or-random-guessing model has been observed and reported for decades. In some of this research, the observation that examinees both guess and make mistakes is related to the study's purpose (e.g., Ebel, 1968;Foster & Ruch, 1927;Little & Creaser, 1968;Melican, Mills, & Plake, 1989;Rowley & Traub, 1977;Sanderson, 1973;Waters, 1967;Wood, 1926). In other cases, this observation is merely incidental (e.g., Bridgeman, 1992;Campbell, 2015;Holzinger, 1924;Jackson, 1955;Michael, Stewart, Douglass, & Rainwater, 1963;Ruch & DeGraff, 1926;Sherriffs, & Boomer, 1954;Slakter, 1968;Swineford & Miller, 1953;Traub, Hambleton, & Singh, 1969;Votow, 1936;Wood, 1976).…”
Section: Knowledge or Guessingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is frequently found that a higher-than-chance proportion of these reluctantly answered items are in fact correct. Thus if either number-right scoring or the conventional guessing correction are used, the subjects increase their scores when forced to respond to items not initially answered (Votaw, 1936;Sherriffs & Boomer, 1954;Waters, 1967;Slakter, 1968a;Pyrczak, 1977;Bliss, 1980). In some of these studies, independent measures of personality were obtained, and it was sometimes found that this affects the degree to which subjects' scores are reduced by reluctance to answer questions they are unsure about, the more submissive or anxious type of personality being the most penalized.…”
Section: Reluctant Responsementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Whether the same subjects are taking similar tests (Hammerton, 1965), or similar subjects are taking the same test (Slakter, 1968b), higher scores are obtained in the latter condition. In addition, Waters (1967) found that the fraction of unanswered items t h a t are correct when an answer is forced is higher in groups which have been most deterred (by knowledge of the scoring formula to be used) from answering.…”
Section: Manipulation Of Instructionsmentioning
confidence: 99%