2003
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.2003.80-43
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Economic and Biological Influences on Key Pecking and Treadle Pressing in Pigeons

Abstract: Pigeons were studied on a two-component multiple schedule in which the required operant was, in different conditions, biologically relevant (i.e., key pecking) or nonbiologically relevant (i.e., treadle pressing). Responding was reinforced on a variable-interval (VI) 2-min schedule in both components. In separate phases, additional food was delivered on a variable-time (VT) 15-s schedule (response independent) or a VI 15-s schedule (response dependent) in one of the components. The addition of response-indepen… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
7
0

Year Published

2006
2006
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
(35 reference statements)
2
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This conclusion is strengthened in light of the fact that the observing response in Experiments 1 and 2 was a treadle press and was a key peck in Experiment 3. Key pecks can be expected to occur at higher rates than treadle presses when maintained by primary reinforcement (e.g., Green & Holt, 2003); the higher rates of treadle pressing obtained in Experiments 1 and 2, therefore, is consistent with the suggestion that the S2 had acquired aversive properties. Similarly, the procedure used in Experiments 1 and 2 (S+ only) may have functioned not as a simple FR 1 that was available intermittently, but more as a general intermittent schedule of observing due to the intervening and unsignaled Ext components during which observing had no consequences.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 71%
“…This conclusion is strengthened in light of the fact that the observing response in Experiments 1 and 2 was a treadle press and was a key peck in Experiment 3. Key pecks can be expected to occur at higher rates than treadle presses when maintained by primary reinforcement (e.g., Green & Holt, 2003); the higher rates of treadle pressing obtained in Experiments 1 and 2, therefore, is consistent with the suggestion that the S2 had acquired aversive properties. Similarly, the procedure used in Experiments 1 and 2 (S+ only) may have functioned not as a simple FR 1 that was available intermittently, but more as a general intermittent schedule of observing due to the intervening and unsignaled Ext components during which observing had no consequences.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 71%
“…Preference reversals also have been obtained in studies with pigeons (Ainslie & Herrnstein, 1981; Calvert, Green, & Myerson, 2010; Green, Fisher, Perlow, & Sherman, 1981; Rachlin & Green, 1972; Rodriguez & Logue, 1988) and rats (Green & Estle, 2003). Green et al (1981) presented pigeons with choices between a smaller, sooner reinforcer and a larger, later reinforcer.…”
Section: Delay Discounting Modelsmentioning
confidence: 75%
“…The majority of discounting studies with animals have examined choices between either different amounts of food (Green et al, 2004; Mazur et al, 1987; Rosati et al, 2007) or different amounts of plain water (Green & Estle, 2003; Reynolds et al, 2002; Richards et al, 1997). However, discounting of other reinforcers also has been studied, including choices involving sucrose (Calvert et al, 2010; Farrar et al, 2003; Freeman et al, 2012), saccharin (Freeman et al, 2009), alcohol (Mitchell et al, 2006; Oberlin & Grahame, 2009), and cocaine (Woolverton, Freeman, Myerson, & Green, 2012; Woolverton et al, 2007).…”
Section: Generality Of Discountingmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In the first A component, he found a typically higher relative level of responding that was maintained over training sessions (see also Williams, 1983). In the last A component, however, he found a higher relative level of responding primarily at the start of the component (an effect known as local contrast-see Terrace, 1966) which was not maintained over training sessions (see also Cleary, 1992;Malone, 1976; but see Green & Holt, 2003). Thus, there is evidence that behavioral contrast may be attributable primarily to the higher rate of responding in anticipation of the poorer schedule, rather than in response to the appearance of the richer schedule (Williams, 1981; see also Williams & Wixted, 1986).…”
Section: Differential or Behavioral Contrastmentioning
confidence: 99%