2014
DOI: 10.1075/lab.4.3.17tsi
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Early, late or very late: Timing acquisition and bilingualism

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

7
115
0
2

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 66 publications
(124 citation statements)
references
References 11 publications
7
115
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…First, greater language exposure should enhance the production of the syntactic options for reference available in the language. This is in line with some recent studies that have emphasized the role of quality and quantity of input in determining (in)complete acquisition of linguistic phenomena (see Kupisch and Rothman 2016;Treffers-Daller et al 2007;Tsimpli 2014). Second, the use of overspecified full DPs should be observable across bilinguals with other language combinations than the ones considered in this study, including bilinguals speaking two non-null subject languages (e.g., English-German) or two null subject and clitic languages (e.g., Italian-Spanish).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 74%
“…First, greater language exposure should enhance the production of the syntactic options for reference available in the language. This is in line with some recent studies that have emphasized the role of quality and quantity of input in determining (in)complete acquisition of linguistic phenomena (see Kupisch and Rothman 2016;Treffers-Daller et al 2007;Tsimpli 2014). Second, the use of overspecified full DPs should be observable across bilinguals with other language combinations than the ones considered in this study, including bilinguals speaking two non-null subject languages (e.g., English-German) or two null subject and clitic languages (e.g., Italian-Spanish).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 74%
“…Verb forms without any inflectional marking have been observed in some eL2 TD children, and have been argued to be covertly finite (Prévost 2003). This agrees with the assumption that knowledge of the relation between verb placement and verbal morphology is not expected to be vulnerable in eL2 acquisition, given that it is in place early in monolingual acquisition (Grimm and Schulz 2016;Tsimpli 2014).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 73%
“…Knowledge of the relation between verb morphology and verb placement is acquired early in monolingual acquisition and is still accessible for second language learners with an age of onset between 2 and 4 (cf. Grimm and Schulz 2016;Tsimpli 2014). Child second language learners with a higher age of onset are hence predicted to violate the ban on non-finite verbs in V2 (for first evidence confirming this prediction see Haberzettl [2005]).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 61%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Our data relate nicely to what is now a large body of evidence, suggesting that mother-tongue literacy plays an important role in overall language development and maintenance as well as academic achievement more globally (Baker 2006;Biber and Hared 1991;Bigelow and Tarone 2004;Eisenchlas, Schalley and Guillemin 2013;Hoff 2006;Tsimpli 2014). Studies investigating bilingual communities in Canada, the United States and Australia show that individuals that are not supported by formal literacy training of some type in the HL in the early years of development face problems maintaining their HL over time (see Cummins 2005;Eisenchlas et al 2013 for review).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 92%