1976
DOI: 10.1080/00140137608931584
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Dynamics of the Eye and Head During an Element of Visual Search

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

1987
1987
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 22 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The present results indicate that this recruitment of larger and more costly muscle groups is likely to increase the reliance on internal processing. As the study of search extends beyond traditional static computer tasks and embraces the full suite of embodied behaviors involved in search (e.g., Gilchrist et al, 2001;Robinson, Koth, & Ringenbach, 1976;Ruddle & Lessels, 2006;Smith, Hood, & Gilchrist, 2008;Solman, Cheyne, & Smilek, 2012;Solman, Wu, Cheyne, & Smilek, 2013;Summala, Pasanen, Räsänen, & Sievänen, 1996;Thomas et al, 2006), consideration of these 1 Adapting the RT reconstruction method used to validate P m estimates in the results section, we estimated the expected eye-contingent repeated search RTs if memory use matched that observed for head-contingent search. In particular, we used head-contingent Initiation Times (h i ) and P m values, eye-contingent random search times (e r ) and decision times (e d ), and a non-zero memory search time chosen as the duration of a typical saccade (t m = 50 ms): RT Ã = h i + e d + (1 À P m ) e r + P m t m .…”
Section: Concluding Commentsmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…The present results indicate that this recruitment of larger and more costly muscle groups is likely to increase the reliance on internal processing. As the study of search extends beyond traditional static computer tasks and embraces the full suite of embodied behaviors involved in search (e.g., Gilchrist et al, 2001;Robinson, Koth, & Ringenbach, 1976;Ruddle & Lessels, 2006;Smith, Hood, & Gilchrist, 2008;Solman, Cheyne, & Smilek, 2012;Solman, Wu, Cheyne, & Smilek, 2013;Summala, Pasanen, Räsänen, & Sievänen, 1996;Thomas et al, 2006), consideration of these 1 Adapting the RT reconstruction method used to validate P m estimates in the results section, we estimated the expected eye-contingent repeated search RTs if memory use matched that observed for head-contingent search. In particular, we used head-contingent Initiation Times (h i ) and P m values, eye-contingent random search times (e r ) and decision times (e d ), and a non-zero memory search time chosen as the duration of a typical saccade (t m = 50 ms): RT Ã = h i + e d + (1 À P m ) e r + P m t m .…”
Section: Concluding Commentsmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…During head movements, users typically move their eyes as well as their heads, such that the eye movement leads the head movement; there is an angular offset between eye orientation and head orientation (Robinson, Koth, & Ringenbach, 1976;Rolland, Ha, & Fidopiastis, 2004;Velger, 1998, p. 173). Thus, more accurate estimates of line of sight are available when both eye movements and head movements are measured (Velger, 1998, p. 173).…”
Section: Head-tracking Accuracymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is especially true when it takes more than a fraction of a second to acquire the peripherally presented information. Robinson, Koth, and Ringenbach (1976) for example demonstrated that when subjects were required to search for a specific digit in a matrix of digits (4 or 8), at target eccentricities of 60, 80, and 100 degrees, head movements occurred 100% of the time; at 40 degrees, approximately 75% of the time; and at eccentricities of less than 20 degrees, less than 2% of the time. The likelihood of head movement was affected by stimulus brightness but not by task difficulty.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%