2019
DOI: 10.1167/19.6.23
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Dynamics of contrast adaptation in central and peripheral vision

Abstract: Adaptation aftereffects are generally stronger for peripheral than for foveal viewing. We examined whether there are also differences in the dynamics of visual adaptation in central and peripheral vision. We tracked the time course of contrast adaptation to binocularly presented Gabor patterns in both the central visual field (within 5°) and in the periphery (beyond 10° eccentricity) using a yes/no detection task to monitor contrast thresholds. Consistent with previous studies, sensitivity losses were stronger… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 60 publications
(83 reference statements)
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Two factors might be responsible for this difference as follows: first, the eccentricity of the probes and, therefore, the activation of more peripheral retinal regions, and second, the distance of the probes to the adapter stimuli. The first hypothesis is likely because visual adaptation is known to generate stronger aftereffects in the periphery, including tilt aftereffects [18,19], motion aftereffects [20,21], shape aftereffects [22,23], face aftereffects [24,25] and contrast aftereffects [26]. Under the second hypothesis, adaptation might be absent (as it was in Experiment 2) because stimuli are presented closer to the adapter, as has already been proposed by [12].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 84%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Two factors might be responsible for this difference as follows: first, the eccentricity of the probes and, therefore, the activation of more peripheral retinal regions, and second, the distance of the probes to the adapter stimuli. The first hypothesis is likely because visual adaptation is known to generate stronger aftereffects in the periphery, including tilt aftereffects [18,19], motion aftereffects [20,21], shape aftereffects [22,23], face aftereffects [24,25] and contrast aftereffects [26]. Under the second hypothesis, adaptation might be absent (as it was in Experiment 2) because stimuli are presented closer to the adapter, as has already been proposed by [12].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 84%
“…The following two factors might play a role: the absolute eccentricity of the probe stimuli or the distance between the adapter and probe stimulus. The first factor, i.e., the absolute eccentricity, is likely relevant because many adaptation effects are stronger in the periphery [18][19][20][21][22][23][24][25][26]. The second factor, i.e., the distance between the adapter and probe stimulus, has been suggested by [12].…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The experimental protocol was the same as that of Gao et al (2019). Specifically, each session lasted 900s and consisted of a 200s pre-adapt, 300s adapt and 400s post-adapt period.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In fact, we are not aware of any robust number adaptation effects that do not depend on the items being presented in the periphery. This point is worth a reflection: There is no obvious reason why number adaptation should be stronger in the periphery, not least because this is (yet again) atypical of visual adaptation in general (e.g., Gao, Webster, & Jiang, 2019;but see, e.g., Zimmermann, 2023).…”
Section: Experiments 2 and 3 -Color And Motionmentioning
confidence: 99%