2010
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.070
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Dynamic brightness induction in V1: Analyzing simulated and empirically acquired fMRI data in a “common brain space” framework

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
21
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 89 publications
1
21
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In general, computational models using multiscale filtering have not been able to explain the same range of brightness percepts as models that do include an explicit surface filling-in mechanisms (Grossberg & Hong, 2006). In a recent largescale computational model, we predicted our dynamic brightness illusion effect in early visual cortex using principles of lateral connectivity as a means for spreading of surface information across the cortex (Peters et al, 2010).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 94%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In general, computational models using multiscale filtering have not been able to explain the same range of brightness percepts as models that do include an explicit surface filling-in mechanisms (Grossberg & Hong, 2006). In a recent largescale computational model, we predicted our dynamic brightness illusion effect in early visual cortex using principles of lateral connectivity as a means for spreading of surface information across the cortex (Peters et al, 2010).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Our results do not exclude the possibility that V1 may also contribute to the perception of brightness (cf. Peters, Jans, van de Ven, De Weerd, & Goebel, 2010;Roe et al, 2005;Ramsden et al, 2001), although this effect may be smaller compared with V2, and our paradigm or stimulus may not have been sensitive enough to pick up the effect in V1. Some fMRI studies, in fact, did not find a brightness correlate in neither V1 nor extrastriate cortex using brightness induction (Cornelissen et al, 2006;Perna et al, 2005) as experimental paradigms.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Importantly, when simulated and measured data co-exist in the same representational space, the same analysis tools (e.g., MVPA, effective connectivity analysis) can be applied to both data sets allowing for quantitative comparisons (Figure 2). See Peters et al (2010) for further details.…”
Section: Integration Of Computational and Experimental Findings In Cbsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the current paper, we highlight recent developments in object recognition research and put forward a “Common Brain Space” framework (CBS; Goebel and De Weerd, 2009; Peters et al, 2010) in which empirical data and computational results can be directly integrated and quantitatively compared.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example Golfinopoulos et al (2010) employ computational models to advance the employment of functional neuroimaging data for the purposes of understanding speech acquisition and production, whereas Schwabe et al (2010) consider models of neuronal firing rates in the visual cortex to explain non-classic extra-receptive field effects of visual contrast. Peters et al (2010) also address the issue of extra-receptive field visual effects and propose a neuroinformatics platform for a common representational "brain space" for the purpose of optimising the confluence of computational and experimental neuroscience. Rigotti et al (2010) consider a mapping between the sequence of attractors expressed in the amygdala and orbito-frontal cortex and a routinized pattern of sensory inputs as the basis for contextual representation of spaces.…”
Section: From Dynamics To Computation and Functionmentioning
confidence: 99%