2001
DOI: 10.1111/desc.2001.4.issue-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Untitled

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
7
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
1
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Already when the typically developed infant is between 3 and 5 months old, parents respond differently to infant vocalizations that are fully voiced and resonant than to vocalizations that are not fully voiced (Hsu, Fogel, & Cooper, 2000). Parents are able to confidently identify well-formed canonical syllables (combinations of consonant-like and vowel-like sounds with a smooth and rapid transition in between, e.g., "da") by the end of the first year (Oller, Eilers, & Basinger, 2001;Ramsdell, Oller, Buder, Ethington, & Chorna, 2012) and differentiate their responses to the child's vocalizations (Gros-Louis et al, 2006). Thus, it seems that parents, in the typical case, respond differently to more "mature" utterances, suggesting that parents also know how to discriminate between utterances at different levels (Gros-Louis et al, 2006).…”
Section: Parental Adaptations To the Childmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Already when the typically developed infant is between 3 and 5 months old, parents respond differently to infant vocalizations that are fully voiced and resonant than to vocalizations that are not fully voiced (Hsu, Fogel, & Cooper, 2000). Parents are able to confidently identify well-formed canonical syllables (combinations of consonant-like and vowel-like sounds with a smooth and rapid transition in between, e.g., "da") by the end of the first year (Oller, Eilers, & Basinger, 2001;Ramsdell, Oller, Buder, Ethington, & Chorna, 2012) and differentiate their responses to the child's vocalizations (Gros-Louis et al, 2006). Thus, it seems that parents, in the typical case, respond differently to more "mature" utterances, suggesting that parents also know how to discriminate between utterances at different levels (Gros-Louis et al, 2006).…”
Section: Parental Adaptations To the Childmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…First, we considered toddlers' manual actions because research suggests that toddlers' object name learning is enhanced when parents name the objects with which their toddlers are manually engaged (Yu & Smith, 2012; see also Scofield, Hernandez-Reif, & Keith, 2009). Second, we considered parents' manual actions based on Gogate's and others' work showing how parents' sensorimotor behaviors as they named objects is associated with better learning (for reviews see Gogate & Hollich, 2010;Gogate, Walker-Andrews, & Bahrick, 2001). Finally, based on a series of recent studies showing that toddlers' visual experiences with objects also shapes learning, we considered toddlers' egocentric object views during play.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In fact, we followed the standard NP paradigm as other related studies did. 9,23,26,38 In the NP paradigm, there are two types of prime-probe pairs: in the test trials, participants need to inhibit the misleading strategy to solve the prime (ie, incongruent task) correctly, and the same strategy needs to be reactivated to solve the subsequent probe (ie, congruent task). In the control trials, the probe is the same task as the one in the test trials; however, the prime is a neutral task involving neither inhibition nor activation of the related strategy; thus, participants' performance on the probes in the control trials could be treated as a baseline.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…That is, if a misleading strategy is inhibited in a prior task, then the subsequent processing of the same strategy in the second task will be impaired. 23 By using the NP paradigm, studies have demonstrated that inhibitory control is needed in solving math problems, such as arithmetic word problems, 38,39 decimal number comparison tasks, 53 geometry comparison tasks, 26 and to overcome the proportional bias. 25,27 Meert, Grégoire and Noël 43 conducted a study with fraction comparison tasks as primes and natural number comparison tasks as probes.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%