2022
DOI: 10.1002/imhj.21982
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Dyadic parent–infant interaction patterns at 12 months: Exploring dyadic parent–infant gender compositions

Abstract: This study investigates differences in dyadic mother-infant and father-infant interaction patterns at infant age 12 months, and the relation between different parent-infant gender compositions and the dyadic interaction. Data were drawn from a large-scale, population-based Norwegian community sample comprising 671 mother-infant and 337 father-infant interactions. The Early Relational Health Screen (ERHS), a screening method for observing dyadic parent-infant interactions, was used to assess the parent-infant i… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 107 publications
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…An interaction was categorized as dyadic when the infant engaged in verbal communication or participated in physical games with a partner that did not involve a third object. Since the data were collected when the infants were mostly preverbal (at 8 and 10 months) and previous research on dyadic interactions has emphasized nonverbal behaviors like bids for attention sharing, affective attunement, imitation, and parent‐child enjoyment in each other (Moore et al., 2013; Provenzi et al., 2018; Sigveland et al., 2022), the present study examined not only verbal communication but also nonverbal interactions interspersed with verbal cues. Examples of dyadic interactions include the mother talking to the infant, the father carrying the infant on his shoulders, the mother stroking the infant's hair, or the grandmother playing peek‐a‐boo with the infant.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…An interaction was categorized as dyadic when the infant engaged in verbal communication or participated in physical games with a partner that did not involve a third object. Since the data were collected when the infants were mostly preverbal (at 8 and 10 months) and previous research on dyadic interactions has emphasized nonverbal behaviors like bids for attention sharing, affective attunement, imitation, and parent‐child enjoyment in each other (Moore et al., 2013; Provenzi et al., 2018; Sigveland et al., 2022), the present study examined not only verbal communication but also nonverbal interactions interspersed with verbal cues. Examples of dyadic interactions include the mother talking to the infant, the father carrying the infant on his shoulders, the mother stroking the infant's hair, or the grandmother playing peek‐a‐boo with the infant.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It addresses the dyad as a unit, not the child's or parent's individual contribution. Parent-child interactions are videorecorded and rated on a 3-point scale (2-clearly observed, 1-sometimes observed, 0-not observed) [40]. The sum score is used, reflecting an overall measure of dyadic interactional quality.…”
Section: Evolution Of Erh Concepts and Practices In The Usmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The BHS evolved to become the Early Relational Health Screen (ERHS) [41]. The ERHS was validated during a population-level Norwegian study [40] and two US studies in primary care and home visiting settings [42]. The BHS and ERHS were developed and validated by White Euro-American researchers with relatively homogeneous samples.…”
Section: Evolution Of Erh Concepts and Practices In The Usmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…At the child level, I controlled for child gender (0 = boy; 1 = girl) because parents may interact differently with their sons versus daughters and boys and girls show differences in language skills (Adani & Cepanec, 2019;Siqveland et al, 2022;Umek et al, 2008). Because children with different temperament may elicit different input from parents and display different language abilities (Conture et al, 2013;Padilla & Ryan, 2019), I also controlled for child temperament, which was measured using maternal report on the EAS Temperament subscale of emotionality (Bus & Plomin, 1984;Buss, 1991).…”
Section: Covariatesmentioning
confidence: 99%