Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology 2021
DOI: 10.1002/9781119684527.ch6
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Does Vocabulary Help Structure the Mind?

Abstract: Does language reflect the categories of our mind or does it help create them? On one widespread view (cognitive priority), learning a language involves mapping words onto pre-existing categories, leaving little room for language to change the structure of conceptual content. On another view (linguistic priority), conceptual structure is shaped by experience with and use of language. We argue for the latter perspective and present experimental findings showing that nameability -the ease with which a feature can… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
8
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 54 publications
(36 reference statements)
2
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This is in line with recent work (e.g. Lupyan, 2012;Lupyan & Zettersten, 2020) suggesting that problem difficulty increases as description length increases and as nameability (the ease with which a concept can be named) decreases; and that language biases perceptual processing towards certain concepts. Furthermore, we find a negative correlation (r=-0.50) between average description length and average accuracy per task.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 91%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This is in line with recent work (e.g. Lupyan, 2012;Lupyan & Zettersten, 2020) suggesting that problem difficulty increases as description length increases and as nameability (the ease with which a concept can be named) decreases; and that language biases perceptual processing towards certain concepts. Furthermore, we find a negative correlation (r=-0.50) between average description length and average accuracy per task.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 91%
“…If so, this poses a challenge for existing LOT-based theories, as it is not clear how the semantics of existing conceptual knowledge could be easily integrated into a minimal LOT. One alternative account that is more parsimonious with our data-especially our findings regarding the relationship between description length and difficulty-is that hypothesis generation uses natural language as a scaffold for generating hypotheses, either instead of or in addition to a symbolic language of thought (Carruthers, 2002;Lupyan & Zettersten, 2020;Andreas, Klein, & Levine, 2017).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 50%
“…It is possible that the larger magnitude of the difference observed here is due to the fact that we tested cultures with large differences in color-term use, and therefore the present results might generalize only to language pairs from vastly different cultures. Both the Whorfian (e.g., Lupyan & Zettersten, 2021) and efficiency-based (e.g., Gibson et al, 2019;Mahowald et al, 2020) approaches predict changes in a native language upon learning a second language. However, a key difference between these approaches is the mechanism behind 4 (green-blue, yellow-brown, red-purple), as well as the reverse of each (i.e., blue-green, brown-yellow, purple-red) across all language groups.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One explanation for the current findings is that labels may be particularly powerful, compact representations of hypotheses about to-be-learned structures (Clark, 1998;Gentner, 2016;Lupyan & Zettersten, 2021). These compact representations (e.g., "alien A likes to eat red things") make it easier for learners to formulate hypotheses about category membership ("it is about the red segment" vs. "it is about that pinkish-purplish segment").…”
Section: Why Are Categories With More Nameable Features Easier To Learn?mentioning
confidence: 85%