2012
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.04.006
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Does valence matter? Effects of negativity on children’s early understanding of the truth and lies

Abstract: Early deceptive behavior often involves acts of wrongdoings on the part of children. As a result, it has often been assumed, although not tested directly, that children are better at identifying lies about wrongdoing than lies about other activities. We tested this assumption in two studies. In Study 1, 67 3- to 5-year-olds viewed vignettes in which a character truthfully or falsely claimed to have committed a good or bad act. Children were biased to label claims that the character had committed a good act as … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
10
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
(46 reference statements)
1
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…They use simple generalized rules to decide what to believe. For example, characters committing a good or bad act are judged as truth-tellers and liars, respectively (Wandrey, Quas, & Lyon, 2012; see also Clément et al, 2004). But if more diagnostic individuating information is available, children as young as 4 years make use of it instead of showing a blanket bias to believe or disbelieve (Brosseau-Liard & Birch, 2011; Brosseau-Liard, Cassels, & Birch, 2014; Robinson et al, 1995; see also Mitchell, Robinson, Rye, & Isaacs, 1997; see also Reyes-Jaquez & Echols, 2015).…”
Section: Integrating Individuating Cues With Contextmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…They use simple generalized rules to decide what to believe. For example, characters committing a good or bad act are judged as truth-tellers and liars, respectively (Wandrey, Quas, & Lyon, 2012; see also Clément et al, 2004). But if more diagnostic individuating information is available, children as young as 4 years make use of it instead of showing a blanket bias to believe or disbelieve (Brosseau-Liard & Birch, 2011; Brosseau-Liard, Cassels, & Birch, 2014; Robinson et al, 1995; see also Mitchell, Robinson, Rye, & Isaacs, 1997; see also Reyes-Jaquez & Echols, 2015).…”
Section: Integrating Individuating Cues With Contextmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, it is much more difficult for children to provide even a minimally competent definition of the truth or a lie compared with identifying whether a statement is true or not (Lyon & Saywitz, 1999; Pipe & Wilson, 1994). Furthermore, children’s apparent understanding will falter if they are asked about truth and lies in contexts in which wrongdoing occurs (because they will err on the side of calling all statements about wrongdoing lies; Wandrey, Quas, & Lyon, 2012). Similarly, a basic understanding of the morality of truth-telling (that true statements are “good” and false statements “bad”) also emerges before children turn 4 (Lyon, Carrick, & Quas, in press), and children’s moral understanding can easily be underestimated.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The majority of research on the development of lying has focused on children's conceptions of and moral reasoning about lying. Three‐year‐olds can recognize statements as lies based on the intentions of the speaker (Bussey, ; Siegal & Peterson, ), but this ability is fragile, and they can still over rely on the factuality and valence of the statement to make their judgment (Wandrey, Quas, & Lyon, ). Gradually over childhood, children get better at distinguishing lies from other types of utterances such as mistakes, exaggerations or jokes such that by early adolescence their understanding resembles adults' (Talwar & Crossman, ).…”
Section: Children's Lie Detectionmentioning
confidence: 99%