2007
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.031
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Does tool-related fMRI activity within the intraparietal sulcus reflect the plan to grasp?

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

15
93
1

Year Published

2008
2008
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 121 publications
(109 citation statements)
references
References 84 publications
15
93
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The modulation of the frontal and posterior N100 response to artifacts is consistent with previous studies that compared the effect of animals and artifacts in visual categorization (Antal et al, 2000(Antal et al, , 2001Proverbio et al, 2007). Furthermore, our posterior N100, peaking around 200 ms, resembles the negative deflection over occipito-temporal areas (N200 or N150) reflecting selective attention effects in target decision tasks using animals (Codispoti et al, 2006;Johnson and Olshausen, 2003) and objects (Van Rullen and Thorpe, 2001). The N150 has been considered to reflect perceptual selection mechanisms devoted to high level features extraction (as those selected in visual categorization) (Codispoti et al, 2006) and, more importantly, it has been demonstrated not to be dependent on low level sensory analysis (Van Rullen and Thorpe, 2001).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The modulation of the frontal and posterior N100 response to artifacts is consistent with previous studies that compared the effect of animals and artifacts in visual categorization (Antal et al, 2000(Antal et al, , 2001Proverbio et al, 2007). Furthermore, our posterior N100, peaking around 200 ms, resembles the negative deflection over occipito-temporal areas (N200 or N150) reflecting selective attention effects in target decision tasks using animals (Codispoti et al, 2006;Johnson and Olshausen, 2003) and objects (Van Rullen and Thorpe, 2001). The N150 has been considered to reflect perceptual selection mechanisms devoted to high level features extraction (as those selected in visual categorization) (Codispoti et al, 2006) and, more importantly, it has been demonstrated not to be dependent on low level sensory analysis (Van Rullen and Thorpe, 2001).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…Furthermore, our posterior N100, peaking around 200 ms, resembles the negative deflection over occipito-temporal areas (N200 or N150) reflecting selective attention effects in target decision tasks using animals (Codispoti et al, 2006;Johnson and Olshausen, 2003) and objects (Van Rullen and Thorpe, 2001). The N150 has been considered to reflect perceptual selection mechanisms devoted to high level features extraction (as those selected in visual categorization) (Codispoti et al, 2006) and, more importantly, it has been demonstrated not to be dependent on low level sensory analysis (Van Rullen and Thorpe, 2001). Considering that the N150 amplitude is larger when stimuli are more difficult to Fig.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 73%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, the danger posed by an animal is not necessarily size-related, nor is the task of inferring intentions and goals. On this functional account, cortical organization may be driven by distinct long-range connections to downstream processes (Mahon and Caramazza, 2011), for example, connecting big object zones to navigational networks (Epstein, 2008), small object zones to dorsal stream reaching regions (Valyear et al, 2007;Bracci et al, 2012), and animate zones to goal-inference or other social regions (Caramazza and Shelton, 1998;Frith, 2007). Alternatively, one could interpret these divisions in terms of the statistical structure in visual/shape properties of these categories.…”
Section: Size Applies Only Within the Inanimate Domainmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Simply viewing pictures of tools was found to activate left parietofrontal areas putatively important for actual tool use (Chao and Martin, 2000;Valyear et al, 2007) and to affect response times to initiate real actions according to affordances defined by both structural and functional tool properties (Bub et al, 2003;Bub et al, 2008). More recently, it was shown that affordance specification for tools includes information about predicted outcomes of actions in accordance with their conventional use (Masson et al, 2011), and reaction times to pantomime tool use with tools in hand are facilitated when preceded by passive viewing of real tools .…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%