2017
DOI: 10.1037/hea0000480
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Does socioeconomic status mediate racial differences in the cortisol response in middle childhood?

Abstract: Objective: Race/ethnicity and socioeconomic status are both associated with stress physiology as indexed by cortisol. The present study tested the extent to which racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol reactivity are explained by socioeconomic status. Method: The sample consisted of 296 racially and socioeconomically diverse children ages 8–11 (47% boys). Mothers reported on children’s stressors and socioeconomic status; salivary cortisol levels were assessed before and after the Trier Social Stress Test. Resul… Show more

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
11
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 25 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
2
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…First, given the higher level of stress often present in low-SES households, the infants in our study may simply have not been as aroused by a research assessment as those from lower-risk households, either due to variation in prenatal programming of the HPA-axis or postnatal environmental differences. Our results are also consistent with prior literature finding an association between low SES and/or parental conflict and low basal cortisol and/or blunted cortisol reactivity in toddlers and children (Sturge-Apple, Davies, Cicchetti, & Manning, 2012; Tackett et al, 2017). Alternatively, about half of the Jansen et al review studies used a heel stick or an inoculation to stress infants which is thought to produce higher initial and peak cortisol levels than, for example, “learning event” studies (e.g., the Still Face Paradigm (SFP) (Thompson et al, 2015, p. 41), which may have inflated those means.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 93%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…First, given the higher level of stress often present in low-SES households, the infants in our study may simply have not been as aroused by a research assessment as those from lower-risk households, either due to variation in prenatal programming of the HPA-axis or postnatal environmental differences. Our results are also consistent with prior literature finding an association between low SES and/or parental conflict and low basal cortisol and/or blunted cortisol reactivity in toddlers and children (Sturge-Apple, Davies, Cicchetti, & Manning, 2012; Tackett et al, 2017). Alternatively, about half of the Jansen et al review studies used a heel stick or an inoculation to stress infants which is thought to produce higher initial and peak cortisol levels than, for example, “learning event” studies (e.g., the Still Face Paradigm (SFP) (Thompson et al, 2015, p. 41), which may have inflated those means.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 93%
“…Keenan et al (2002; 2007) based upon the same sample, used the Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale (NBAS) (a measure similar to the one used in the present study, but more cumbersome and harder to administer) and a heel stick to stress infants within the first two days of life, but data collected from infants so close to the stressful experience of birth raises the possibility of confounded results (Mears, McAuliffe, Grimes, & Morrison, 2004). Although the majority of studies examined infant basal cortisol, since 2010 a number of other major studies or reviews about infant cortisol reactivity have been published focusing on a variety of variables including preterm infants (Morelius et al, 2016), sex differences in childhood (van der Voorn, Hollanders, Ket, Rotteveel, & Finken, 2017), adverse experiences (Hunter et al, 2011), mother-infant adrenocortical attunement (Hibel et al, 2015), or SES and race/ethnicity in older children (Tackett, Herzhoff, Smack, Reardon, & Adam, 2017).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Sample 1 is originally described in Tackett (2011), and all families from the original sample were included here. Sample 2 is originally described in Tackett, Herzhoff, Smack, Reardon, and Adam (2017), and all but one family (missing all relevant variables) from the original sample were included here. Participants were 711 primary caregivers of 695 7-to-13-year-old children (mean age = 9.87 years; 48% male).…”
Section: Participantsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Participants were from families with middle to upper-middle socioeconomic status (SES), as 80.9% of the fathers and 75.9% of the mothers obtained a bachelor's degree or above and 58.2% of the families had a household income of more than 10,000¥ per month. In accordance with prior studies (Cohen, Doyle, & Baum, 2006;Schulting, Malone, & Dodge, 2005;Tackett, Herzhoff, Smack, Reardon, & Adam, 2017), participants' SES was calculated by summing the standard scores for the following three variables: (a) father's education (M = 4.80, SD = 0.75), (b) mother's education (M = 4.72, SD = 0.76), and (c) household income (M = 3.64, SD = 1.00).…”
Section: Participantsmentioning
confidence: 99%