2017
DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2017.1323813
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Does self-prioritization affect perceptual processes?

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

6
54
1
4

Year Published

2018
2018
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 37 publications
(69 citation statements)
references
References 54 publications
6
54
1
4
Order By: Relevance
“…Inter-estingly, no differences in the efficiency of stimulus processing were observed as a function of ownership. Together with related research, this reveals that, rather than enhancing stimulus salience, self-relevance expedites performance through its influence on post-perceptual (i.e., decisional) processing operations ( Miyakoshi et al, 2007 ; Reuther & Chakravarthi, 2017 ; Siebold et al, 2015 ; Stein et al, 2016 ; Wade & Vickery, 2018 ).…”
Section: Ownership and Decisional Processingmentioning
confidence: 52%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Inter-estingly, no differences in the efficiency of stimulus processing were observed as a function of ownership. Together with related research, this reveals that, rather than enhancing stimulus salience, self-relevance expedites performance through its influence on post-perceptual (i.e., decisional) processing operations ( Miyakoshi et al, 2007 ; Reuther & Chakravarthi, 2017 ; Siebold et al, 2015 ; Stein et al, 2016 ; Wade & Vickery, 2018 ).…”
Section: Ownership and Decisional Processingmentioning
confidence: 52%
“…The putative exclusivity of the self-ownership effect derives, at least in part, from its underlying origin. Despite the contention that self-relevance facilitates perceptual processing ( Humphreys & Sui, 2016 ; Sui & Humphreys, 2015 , 2017 ; Sui & Rotshtein, 2019 ; but see Reuther & Chakravarthi, 2017 ), studies manipulating object ownership have garnered little support for this viewpoint. Instead, self-prioritisation has been traced to the operation of a different underlying mechanism—a response bias ( Constable et al, 2019 ).…”
Section: Ownership and Decisional Processingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The results showed that if the pairings were matching, then participants were faster to judge self-associated than friend-or stranger-associated stimuli, suggesting that participants demonstrated a "self-prioritization effect" for arbitrary rapidly self-associated geometrical shapes. This effect was later replicated with geometrical shapes [22][23][24][25][26][27][28][29][30][31][32][33][34], other kinds of visual stimuli, such as Gabor patches [35], tilted lines [36], images of food [37], avatars [38], and faces [20,39], as well as with stimuli in other sensory modalities [40][41][42].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 82%
“…This self-prioritization effect during perceptual matching is maintained throughout the life span (Sui & Humphreys, 2017). Additional findings indicate that self-association modulates access to visual awareness under continuous flash suppression , and the effects are more pronounced in explicit (e.g., self-relevant) than implicit tasks (e.g., self-irrelevant, judging the orientation of stimuli; Falbén et al, 2019;Reuther & Chakravarthi, 2017). Evidence from mathematical modeling analysis has further shown that self-association changes particular functional processes Sui, Enock, Ralph, & Humphreys, 2015).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 83%