2001
DOI: 10.1016/s0885-2014(01)00066-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Does half a pizza equal half a box of chocolates?

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
46
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
3
3
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 89 publications
(54 citation statements)
references
References 11 publications
5
46
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Our results support previous findings (Schlottmann, 2001;Singer-Freeman & Goswami, 2001) indicating that proportional reasoning is possible early in life when using an intuitive task; nevertheless 5-year-olds were more accurate than 4-year-olds in rating proportions. In line with previous results (Frick & Newcombe, 2012), a similar age effect was found for children's ability to locate targets presented on maps.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Our results support previous findings (Schlottmann, 2001;Singer-Freeman & Goswami, 2001) indicating that proportional reasoning is possible early in life when using an intuitive task; nevertheless 5-year-olds were more accurate than 4-year-olds in rating proportions. In line with previous results (Frick & Newcombe, 2012), a similar age effect was found for children's ability to locate targets presented on maps.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…On the one hand, 5-year-olds can successfully rate probabilities of events on a continuous scale (Schlottmann, 2001), and 3-and 4-year-olds can match proportions across substances (e.g., half a pizza equals half a chocolate bar; Singer-Freeman & Goswami, 2001). On the other hand, same-aged children had difficulties in finding the matching proportion between two alternatives (Boyer & Levine, 2012;Spinillo & Bryant, 1991).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Findings suggested that children as young as 8 years old were able to consider both components that constitute a proportion and integrate them in a normative proportional way 2 . These results stand in contrast to previous claims that proportional reasoning does not emerge before the age of 11 years (Moore et al, 1991;Noelting, 1980;Piaget & Inhelder, 1975) and confirm other findings that even younger children are able to reason about proportions (Acredolo et al, 1989;Boyer & Levine, 2012;Boyer et al, 2008;Jeong et al, 2007;Schlottmann, 2001;Singer-Freeman & Goswami, 2001;Sophian, 2000;Spinillo & Bryant, 1991, 1999. In line with previous paradigms showing earlier success in children's proportional reasoning, it is possible that the presentation of continuous proportional quantities and the nature of the response mode (spatial ratings that are more intuitively graspable) led to children's success on our proportional reasoning task.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 71%
“…That is, children's formal fraction knowledge may have helped them to encode spatial proportions and to reproduce them on the rating scale. Even though this possibility cannot be eliminated by our correlational results, it seems unlikely in light of many studies (Acredolo et al, 1989;Boyer & Levine, 2012;Boyer et al, 2008;Jeong et al, 2007;Schlottmann, 2001;Singer-Freeman & Goswami, 2001;Sophian, 2000;Spinillo & Bryant, 1991, 1999 showing signs of proportional reasoning at an age when understanding of formal fractions is not present (Hecht & Vagi, 2010;Schneider & Siegler, 2010;Stafylidou & Vosniadou, 2004). Nonetheless, future studies using longitudinal designs or training components are needed to pin down the causal direction of the relation we have identified.…”
Section: The Relation Between Proportional Reasoning and Fraction Undmentioning
confidence: 79%
See 1 more Smart Citation