2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.06.006
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Does children’s moral compass waver under social pressure? Using the conformity paradigm to test preschoolers’ moral and social-conventional judgments

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

6
32
2

Year Published

2017
2017
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 37 publications
(43 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
6
32
2
Order By: Relevance
“…To conclude, we presented children and adolescents with a comic character suggesting how goods should be shared. This simple manipulation had a rather strong influence on participants' actual sharing decisions, similar to previous research that indicated that children readily learn from and conform to information presented by real-life or videotaped models, puppets, or drawings (e.g., Benozio & Diesendruck, 2016;Kim et al, 2016;Wood, Kendal, & Flynn, 2013b). Consistent with social learning theory (Bandura, 1977), our results demonstrate the power of models to influence children's and adolescents' fairness-related behavior, showing for the first time that the magnitude of this power depends on the match between model type (e.g., adult or peer) and developmental phase (e.g., child or adolescent).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 79%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…To conclude, we presented children and adolescents with a comic character suggesting how goods should be shared. This simple manipulation had a rather strong influence on participants' actual sharing decisions, similar to previous research that indicated that children readily learn from and conform to information presented by real-life or videotaped models, puppets, or drawings (e.g., Benozio & Diesendruck, 2016;Kim et al, 2016;Wood, Kendal, & Flynn, 2013b). Consistent with social learning theory (Bandura, 1977), our results demonstrate the power of models to influence children's and adolescents' fairness-related behavior, showing for the first time that the magnitude of this power depends on the match between model type (e.g., adult or peer) and developmental phase (e.g., child or adolescent).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 79%
“…Based on Asch's () conformity experiments, preschoolers were presented with a peer majority who made unanimous but erroneous judgments. Of 4‐year‐olds, 40%–60% adapted their responses to those of the (incorrect) peer majority on visual and social‐conventional judgment tasks, despite knowing the correct solution (e.g., Haun & Tomasello, ; Kim, Chen, Smetana, & Greenberger, ). However, 60%–80% of preschoolers did not conform to a peer (or adult) majority when conformity contradicted a moral norm (Enesco, Sebastián‐Enesco, Guerrero, Quan, & Garijo, ; Engelmann, Herrmann, Rapp, & Tomasello, ).…”
Section: Influence Of Othersmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In an environment that is rife with unethical behaviour, it becomes increasingly difficult to make ethical decisions, or to objectively assess the implications of a decision (Van der Walt, Jonck & Sobayeni 2016). The findings relating to work-related scenarios however confirm claims that individuals generally develop a moral compass as they grow older (Kim et al 2016). This finding also resonates with Van der Walt et al's (2016) findings that there were statistically significant generational differences amongst Baby Boomers, Generation X and Generation Y for certain facets of work ethics such as hard work and delay of gratification.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 83%
“…Although the paradigm used here is different from the one normally used in the field of social moral reasoning, our participants' choices across the trials suggest that they perceived conventions to be more modifiable than moral norms. Within a social conformity paradigm, Kim and colleagues () also found that children were much less susceptible to peer pressure when the issue dealt with hitting or teasing another child than when it had to do with unconventional actions (e.g., wearing a bathing suit to day care).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In contrast to most of the studies on testimonial information, here we presented a unanimous majority composed by children of the same age as the participants. The few works in this regard show that children are sensitive to consensus information provided by peers, and, in some cases, side with them even when their opinion is in conflict with the children's perceptual experience or the normative judgments about social events (Haun & Tomasello, ; Kim, Chen, Smetana, & Greenberger, ). This tendency to conform to other children's opinions seems to follow a social motivation, the aim of being accepted into the peer group, what is called normative conformity (Claidière & Whiten, ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%