2020
DOI: 10.1111/desc.12943
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Do you understand what I want to tell you? Early sensitivity in bilinguals' iconic gesture perception and production

Abstract: Previous research has shown differences in monolingual and bilingual communication. We explored whether monolingual and bilingual pre-schoolers (N = 80) differ in their ability to understand others' iconic gestures (gesture perception) and produce intelligible iconic gestures themselves (gesture production) and how these two abilities are related to differences in parental iconic gesture frequency. In a gesture perception task, the experimenter replaced the last word of every sentence with an iconic gesture. T… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

2
12
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 67 publications
(108 reference statements)
2
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Developmental studies suggest that children might use gestures as an alternative channel of expression to compensate for their limited linguistic proficiency (e.g., younger vs. older children or children vs. adults; Church et al, 2000 ; Alibali et al, 2009 ; Colletta et al, 2010 ). This is in line with bilingualism research showing that bilingual children speaking in their L2 used more gestures than monolinguals (e.g., Smithson et al, 2011 ; Wermelinger et al, 2020 ). Moreover, research on clinical populations with communication and language delays suggests that although there are delays in gesture production in the first 2 years, gesture might be used to compensate for communication and language difficulties at preschool and school ages by some children ( Özçalışkan et al, 2013 ; LeBarton and Iverson, 2017 ).…”
Section: Individual Differences In Gesture Productionsupporting
confidence: 89%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Developmental studies suggest that children might use gestures as an alternative channel of expression to compensate for their limited linguistic proficiency (e.g., younger vs. older children or children vs. adults; Church et al, 2000 ; Alibali et al, 2009 ; Colletta et al, 2010 ). This is in line with bilingualism research showing that bilingual children speaking in their L2 used more gestures than monolinguals (e.g., Smithson et al, 2011 ; Wermelinger et al, 2020 ). Moreover, research on clinical populations with communication and language delays suggests that although there are delays in gesture production in the first 2 years, gesture might be used to compensate for communication and language difficulties at preschool and school ages by some children ( Özçalışkan et al, 2013 ; LeBarton and Iverson, 2017 ).…”
Section: Individual Differences In Gesture Productionsupporting
confidence: 89%
“…Gesture is also an early diagnostic tool to foresee persistent language delay, especially for children with unilateral brain lesions ( Sauer et al, 2010 ; Özçalışkan et al, 2013 ). Although these studies suggest a link between early spoken language abilities and gesture production in children, the direct evidence on how individual differences in early receptive and expressive language skills relate with spontaneous gesture use within children with and without language delays is quite limited ( Kartalkanat and Göksun, 2020 ; Wermelinger et al, 2020 ).…”
Section: Individual Differences In Gesture Productionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Here, we test whether bilingual caregivers adjust their gestures depending on addressee (toddler-vs adult-directed) and cognitive-linguistic context (monolingual vs. bilingual/synonym condition). First, given reports in the literature that bilinguals tend to gesture more than monolinguals (Gullberg, 2013;Nicoladis et al, 2007;Wermelinger et al, 2020), we expect to see a higher gesture rate in bilingual caregivers compared to monolingual caregivers across all conditions. Second, if caregivers believe that a bilingual context (when both languages are used) is more difficult for a child than a monolingual context, they might compensate for this by using more gestures.…”
Section: Current Studymentioning
confidence: 89%
“…In addition, several studies reported that bilinguals gesture more than monolinguals, which has been associated with weaker language proficiency, or alternatively, might be due to more general increased cognitive load associated with bilingual language use (Alibali et al, 1997;Ping & Goldin-Meadow, 2010;Smithson & Nicoladis, 2013). Furthermore, in a gesture reproduction task that required participants describing different actions in the language of their choice to an adult, Swiss German bilinguals produced more iconic gestures than Swiss-German monolinguals (Wermelinger et al, 2020). To gain further insight into gesture production in bilinguals and its relation with communicative and cognitive-linguistic demands, the current study investigated gesture use in story-retelling by monolingual and bilingual caregivers.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The form of the gesture depicts some characteristic or attribute of the referent. Gestures such as flapping the hands to refer to a bird, or pretending to drink without a real cup, are examples of this type of gesture that have been called: "enactive gestures" (Zinober & Martlew, 1985), "characterizing gestures" (Goldin-Meadow & Morford, 1990), "symbolic gestures" (Bates et al, 1979(Bates et al, , 1980Fasolo & D'Odorico, 2012), "representational gestures" (Batista et al, 2019;Rowe et al, 2008), or "iconic gestures" (Özçalışkan & Goldin-Meadow, 2009;Wermelinger et al, 2020).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%