2021
DOI: 10.1016/j.erap.2021.100623
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Do not interrupt me if it makes me feel something – Study of the effect of the pleasantness of interruptions on performance

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Most studies carried out in natural environments (6/10) allowed for search resumption, however this was always dependent on the completion of a task during the interruption ( Williams and Drew, 2017 ; Drew et al, 2018 ; Wynn et al, 2018 ; Brazzolotto and Michael, 2020 , 2021 ; Radović et al, 2022 ). In natural scenarios where the interruption ended the search, participants were required to complete a task in two studies ( Rice and Trafimow, 2012 ; Nachtnebel et al, 2023 ) while in one study, they were not required to perform any task post-interruption ( Rieger et al, 2021 ).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…Most studies carried out in natural environments (6/10) allowed for search resumption, however this was always dependent on the completion of a task during the interruption ( Williams and Drew, 2017 ; Drew et al, 2018 ; Wynn et al, 2018 ; Brazzolotto and Michael, 2020 , 2021 ; Radović et al, 2022 ). In natural scenarios where the interruption ended the search, participants were required to complete a task in two studies ( Rice and Trafimow, 2012 ; Nachtnebel et al, 2023 ) while in one study, they were not required to perform any task post-interruption ( Rieger et al, 2021 ).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When we assessed whether the interruption event was considered a task or not, we observed that 14 studies did not involve a task as the interruption ( Olds et al, 2000a , 2000b , 2000c , 2001 ; Olds and Punambolam, 2002 ; Lleras et al, 2005 , 2007 ; Van Zoest et al, 2007 ; Jungé et al, 2009 ; Lleras and Enns, 2009 ; Thomas and Lleras, 2009 ; Godwin et al, 2013 ; Mereu et al, 2014 ; Rieger et al, 2021 ); and all but one of these studies ( Rieger et al, 2021 ) were conducted in artificial environments. In contrast, in the 13 studies where the interruption was a task, five were conducted in artificial environments ( Beck et al, 2006 ; Ratwani and Trafton, 2008 ; Höfler et al, 2011 ; Alonso et al, 2021 ; Labonté and Vachon, 2021 ) and eight in natural ones ( Rice and Trafimow, 2012 ; Williams and Drew, 2017 ; Drew et al, 2018 ; Wynn et al, 2018 ; Brazzolotto and Michael, 2020 , 2021 ; Radović et al, 2022 ; Nachtnebel et al, 2023 ). Within these 13 studies, two involved interrupting event tasks that were directly related to the interrupted search, requiring participants to use information obtained during the incomplete search to complete their tasks ( Beck et al, 2006 ; Rice and Trafimow, 2012 ) whereas the tasks in the remaining nine studies were search-unrelated.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Interruptions during search seem to increase search times (e.g. 8 , 30 , 31 ), although participants can quite well resume search after a brief interruption 32 . The complete abortion of a search (i.e., when the interruption occurs with no possibility to continue searching afterwards) often goes along with time pressure tasks.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%