2019
DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2019.1577756
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Do learners declining to seek help conform to rational principles?

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…While in another two studies, researchers found gender is not a variable that impacts help-seeking [ 80 ]. In another experimental research, Miranda Lery Santos et al [ 81 ] compare the economic, time, and social costs of help-seeking to the expected benefits, and found participants were more likely to seek help when there was no economic cost to help, but were not as sensitive to the time cost and social cost parameters.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While in another two studies, researchers found gender is not a variable that impacts help-seeking [ 80 ]. In another experimental research, Miranda Lery Santos et al [ 81 ] compare the economic, time, and social costs of help-seeking to the expected benefits, and found participants were more likely to seek help when there was no economic cost to help, but were not as sensitive to the time cost and social cost parameters.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Considerations that might encourage help seeking include the wish to provide informative answers (Grice 1975, see also Goldsmith, 2016, satisfying one's curiosity (Berlyne & Frommer, 1966;Ferguson et al, 2015;Son & Metcalfe, 2000), and receiving feedback about the accuracy of one's knowledge (Bamberger, 2009;Nelson-Le Gall & Jones, 1990). At the same time, the wish to save time, to maintain a sense of independence and selfreliance, and to avoid failing after receiving help might discourage help seeking (Arbreton, 1998;Butler, 1998Butler, , 2006Metcalfe, 2009;Santos et al, 2020;Nadler, 2017;Newman & Goldin, 1990;Undorf & Ackerman, 2017). Moreover, the weaker association between confidence and help seeking than between confidence and withholding answers is consistent with the large body of work showing that help seeking involves various sub-processes and is affected by multiple individual differences, situational variables, and cultural factors (e.g., Butler, 2013;Nadler, 2017;Newman, 2000;Volet & Karabenick, 2006).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In contrast, people might seek instrumental help not only to improve the immediate accuracy of answers, but also in order to improve the informativeness of answers, satisfy curiosity (Berlyne & Frommer, 1966;Ferguson et al, 2015;Son & Metcalfe, 2000), or receive feedback about the accuracy of their knowledge (Bamberger, 2009;Nelson-Le Gall & Jones, 1990). At the same time, people may be hesitant to seek help for various reasons, including an orientation toward independence and self-reliance (Butler, 1998;Nadler, 2017;Ryan et al, 2009), experiencing help seeking as a threat to self-esteem (Butler, 2006;Halabi & Nadler, 2017;Karabenick & Berger, 2013;Nadler, 1991Nadler, , 2017, and reluctance to spend more time on task (Arbreton, 1998;Metcalfe, 2009;Santos et al, 2020;Newman & Goldin, 1990;Undorf & Ackerman, 2017). Consistent with the idea that improving immediate accuracy is not the sole impetus for help seeking, found stronger relationships between confidence and help seeking in children when using a contingency reward system that encouraged limiting help seeking to cases in which participants thought that they needed help to answer correctly.…”
Section: The Present Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Here, people’s behavior may align less closely with their metacognitive monitoring; although people are broadly more likely to consult external aids when less confident (Cotler et al, 1970 , EL: 5; Nelson & Fyfe, 2019 ; EL: 5; Undorf et al, 2021 , EL: 3), they sometimes do not seek help even when low in confidence (Undorf et al, 2021 , EL: 3). One reason for this may be that seeking external help incurs additional costs, such as requiring more time or—in the case of asking another person—social judgment from one’s peers or supervisors (Halabi & Nadler, 2017 , EL: 2; Karabenick & Gonida, 2018 , EL: 3; Nadler, 1991 , EL: 3; Nadler, 2017 , EL: 3; Nadler & Chernyak-Hai, 2014 , EL: 3, but see Miranda Lery Santos et al, 2020 , EL: 4, for null effects of the time taken to request help). Such negative consequences of help-seeking may be particularly strong for individuals from socially disadvantaged groups, for whom help-seeking may be viewed as reinforcing negative stereotypes of inability or dependence (Halabi et al, 2016 , EL: 5; Halabi & Nadler, 2017 , EL: 2; Nadler, 2017 , EL: 3; Nadler & Chernyak-Hai, 2014 , EL: 3).…”
Section: Metacognitive Monitoring Can Be Reasonably Accuratementioning
confidence: 99%