2015
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0126203
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Distractor Suppression When Attention Fails: Behavioral Evidence for a Flexible Selective Attention Mechanism

Abstract: Despite consistent evidence showing that attention is a multifaceted mechanism that can operate at multiple levels of processing depending on the structure and demands of the task, investigations of the attentional blink phenomenon have consistently shown that the impairment in reporting the second of two targets typically occurs at a late, or post-perceptual, stage of processing. This suggests that the attentional blink phenomenon may represent the operation of a unique attentional mechanism that is not as fl… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 43 publications
(70 reference statements)
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, note that this correlation may be spurious because these two indices share a variable (i.e., T2/T1 at Lag 4 in no-prime trials). Nonindependent variables such as AB recovery and T2 priming can be expected to show an average correlation of .50 (in case of equal variances), even in the complete absence of a true relationship (see Elliott & Giesbrecht, 2015). Moreover, Slagter and Georgopoulou (2013) did not observe a relationship between distractor inhibition and AB magnitude (i.e., T2/ T1 at Lag 10 − Lag 2).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…However, note that this correlation may be spurious because these two indices share a variable (i.e., T2/T1 at Lag 4 in no-prime trials). Nonindependent variables such as AB recovery and T2 priming can be expected to show an average correlation of .50 (in case of equal variances), even in the complete absence of a true relationship (see Elliott & Giesbrecht, 2015). Moreover, Slagter and Georgopoulou (2013) did not observe a relationship between distractor inhibition and AB magnitude (i.e., T2/ T1 at Lag 10 − Lag 2).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…Note, however, that this correlation may be spurious because these two indices share a variable (i.e., T2/T1 at Lag 4 in no-prime trials). Nonindependent variables such as AB recovery and T2 priming can be expected to show an average correlation of .50 (in case of equal variances), even in the complete absence of a true relationship (Archie, 1981; see also Elliott & Giesbrecht, 2015). Moreover, Slagter and Georgopoulou (2013) did not observe a relationship between distractor inhibition and AB magnitude (i.e., T2/T1 at Lag 10 -Lag 2).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, these individual differences reliably correlated with the AB magnitude (those with smaller ABs had negative priming) and T1 performance (those with high T1 accuracy had negative priming). The authors viewed attention as the causal factor of these individual differences, with differences in distractor inhibition during the blink causing priming differences (however, see Elliott & Giesbrecht, 2015 for a failure to replicate this correlation result and a computer simulation suggesting that the previously reported correlation may have been an artifact when using the same data twice). If this correlation exists, the perceptual wink model provides a qualitatively different account, with differences in perception as the causal factor: individuals with more rapid orthographic perception more easily perceive T1, more readily experience letter habituation (i.e., negative priming) and more rapidly recover the ability to detect the existence of a second target (i.e., revealed as a small AB magnitude).…”
Section: A Comparison With Other Priming-ab Studiesmentioning
confidence: 99%