2019
DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2018.1561568
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Distractor-interference reduction is dimensionally constrained

Abstract: The dimension-weighting account predicts that if observers search for a target standing out from the background in a particular dimension, they cannot readily ignore a distractor standing out in the same dimension. This prediction is tested here by asking two groups of observers to search for an orientation target or a luminance target, respectively, and presenting an additional distractor defined in either the respectively same dimension or the other dimension. Notably, in this cross-over design, the physical… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
40
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

5
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 31 publications
(42 citation statements)
references
References 77 publications
2
40
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Testing this quantitative prediction is complicated by the fact that—because of up-weighting of the target dimension—the same-dimension distractor necessarily has a higher priority (and therefore causes more interference) than the different-dimension distractor to begin with (Liesefeld et al, 2019; Liesefeld & Müller, 2019). Multiplying with the same (spatial weighting) factor has a larger effect on higher than on lower values 2 .…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Testing this quantitative prediction is complicated by the fact that—because of up-weighting of the target dimension—the same-dimension distractor necessarily has a higher priority (and therefore causes more interference) than the different-dimension distractor to begin with (Liesefeld et al, 2019; Liesefeld & Müller, 2019). Multiplying with the same (spatial weighting) factor has a larger effect on higher than on lower values 2 .…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Targets and distractors always appeared on the second ring from fixation except for the midline vertical (12 and 6 o’clock) locations; that is, there were five possible locations per display side (relevant regions). The exclusion of midline positions and the crucial distractor-location manipulation were the only notable deviations from Liesefeld et al (2019).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 97%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Ultimately, of course, it is the priority map via which the inhibition is always expressed in search guidance, but the true level, at which it is instantiated at least in certain conditions, may be below the priority map. This is as envisaged by the dimension-weighting account, according to which selection is ultimately based on the priority map which, however, is itself shaped by the weighting applied to the various, target- and distractor-defining feature dimensions (e.g., Liesefeld, Liesefeld, & Müller, 2019; for a recent review, see Liesefeld, Liesefeld, Pollmann, & Müller, in press).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Motor conflicts occur when we must deviate from a prepotent response pattern, such as when drivers need to hit the brakes because of an unexpected stop sign (Botvinick et al, 2004;Egner, 2008;Aron, 2011;Cohen, 2014a). Both attentional and motor conflicts lead to increased error rates and slower reaction times (Criaud and Boulinguez, 2013;Gaspelin and Luck, 2018a;Wessel, 2018;Liesefeld et al, 2019). To understand how the brain enables top-down control of our behavior, it is crucial to find out whether different types of conflicts are resolved by separate, domain-specific or shared, domain-general top-down control mechanisms in the brain (Cocchi et al, 2013;Mackie et al, 2013;Hampshire and Sharp, 2015;Gratton et al, 2018).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%