2013
DOI: 10.1037/a0032880
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Distinguishing primary and secondary variants of callous-unemotional traits among adolescents in a clinic-referred sample.

Abstract: The current study used model-based cluster analyses to determine if there are two distinct variants of adolescents (ages 11 - 18) high on callous-unemotional (CU) traits that differ on their level of anxiety and history of trauma. The sample (n = 272) consisted of clinic-referred youths who were primarily African-American (90%) and from low income families. Consistent with hypotheses, three clusters emerged, including a group low on CU traits, as well as two groups high on CU traits that differed in their leve… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

17
105
2

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
2

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 125 publications
(133 citation statements)
references
References 93 publications
17
105
2
Order By: Relevance
“…It is possible that the increased attention to distress cues noted among CU youth experiencing anxiety represents a sensitivity to negative emotional cues in general, similar to the attentional biases towards negative-and threat-related cues (Mogg & Bradley, 2005;Reid et al, 2006), and bias towards interpreting ambiguous information negatively (Taghavi et al, 2000) documented among anxious children. While CU traits have not previously been associated with a hostile attribution bias (HAB; Dodge, Price, Bachorowski, & Newman, 1990;Frick et al, 2003a;Helseth, Waschbusch, King, & Willoughby, 2015), perhaps the combination of cognitive biases associated with anxiety and the callousness of CU traits yields the impulsive, aggressive reactivity documented in these youth (e.g., Fanti et al, 2013;Kahn et al, 2013).…”
Section: Aggressionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 4 more Smart Citations
“…It is possible that the increased attention to distress cues noted among CU youth experiencing anxiety represents a sensitivity to negative emotional cues in general, similar to the attentional biases towards negative-and threat-related cues (Mogg & Bradley, 2005;Reid et al, 2006), and bias towards interpreting ambiguous information negatively (Taghavi et al, 2000) documented among anxious children. While CU traits have not previously been associated with a hostile attribution bias (HAB; Dodge, Price, Bachorowski, & Newman, 1990;Frick et al, 2003a;Helseth, Waschbusch, King, & Willoughby, 2015), perhaps the combination of cognitive biases associated with anxiety and the callousness of CU traits yields the impulsive, aggressive reactivity documented in these youth (e.g., Fanti et al, 2013;Kahn et al, 2013).…”
Section: Aggressionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Anxiety may interact with CU in important ways, but the nature of such interactions and their impact on youth aggressive behavior remain unclear, impeding the identification of tailored treatment targets, and hindering efforts to address the unique needs of children at various points on these continua. Moreover, and importantly, research examining interactions between CU and anxiety and their effects on AB has relied almost exclusively on questionnaire reports of child aggressive behavior (e.g., Fanti et al, 2013;Kahn et al, 2013, Rosan et al, 2015, and the studies that used criminal records did not assess aggression in an experimental context (e.g., Kimonis et al, 2011;Lee et al, 2010), which limits interpretations and cannot rule out issues related to reporter biases, shared method variance, and external circumstances.…”
Section: Cu and Anxietymentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations