2015
DOI: 10.1037/pag0000057
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Dissociative global and local task-switching costs across younger adults, middle-aged adults, older adults, and very mild Alzheimer’s disease individuals.

Abstract: A task-switching paradigm was used to examine differences in attentional control across younger adults, middle-aged adults, healthy older adults, and individuals classified in the earliest detectable stage of Alzheimer's disease (AD). A large sample of participants (570) completed a switching task in which participants were cued to classify the letter (consonant/vowel) or number (odd/even) task-set dimension of a bivalent stimulus (e.g., A 14), respectively. A Pure block consisting of single-task trials and a … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

8
20
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 25 publications
(28 citation statements)
references
References 60 publications
8
20
0
Order By: Relevance
“…A predicted cubic age relationship would indicate a developmental improvement in EF in adolescence through to young adulthood, a decline in EF across adulthood, and a further steeper decline in EF in older age. Finally, in line with previous research (e.g., 84 ) we predicted that the different aspects of cognitive flexibility would should show distinct effects: we predicted that switching costs (i.e., changing task sets) would not show any age-related changes, but mixing costs (i.e., maintaining multiple task sets) would show an increase across adulthood (e.g., 84,85 ).…”
supporting
confidence: 82%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…A predicted cubic age relationship would indicate a developmental improvement in EF in adolescence through to young adulthood, a decline in EF across adulthood, and a further steeper decline in EF in older age. Finally, in line with previous research (e.g., 84 ) we predicted that the different aspects of cognitive flexibility would should show distinct effects: we predicted that switching costs (i.e., changing task sets) would not show any age-related changes, but mixing costs (i.e., maintaining multiple task sets) would show an increase across adulthood (e.g., 84,85 ).…”
supporting
confidence: 82%
“…Mixing costs have generally been found to be greater in older adults (e.g., [54][55][56][57][58] ) there are mixed results in regard to switch costs, with some studies reporting an age-related increase (e.g., 59 ), a U-shaped trajectory 53 or no age-related differences (e.g., 58,59 ), most likely due to differences in the task switching paradigms. We note that the current study used an alternating-runs paradigm without a preparatory cue-stimulus interval, which is analogous to Huff et al 's 84 task-switching paradigm with comparable aging results. In addition, switch and mixing costs showed a negative correlation, reflecting that individuals with a greater switch cost also had a reduced mixing cost, and vice versa, and this pattern also remained when accounting for the effect of age.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 84%
“…Furthermore, elaborations of designs and modeling strategy have furrowed the notion of switching costs into ever finer scales of time and rooted them in finer subdivisions of the cognitive system. For instance, as experimenters have tested task switching not just across block but also within block, they need to distinguish between global and local costs of switching, respectively [82] or, by another-yet-synonymous set of labels, switching costs and mixing costs, respectively [83]. Local/mixing costs may reflect that trial-by-trial variation in the task set never permits full reconfiguring the task set, and more global/switching costs may come from maintaining multiple tasks sets [82], but the costs of having to inhibit interfering task-stimulus relationships seem to accrue across time, no matter their source [84].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition to the neuropsychological test batteries, participants also completed a battery of computerized attentional (Spieler, Balota, & Faust, 1996;Stroop, 1935), a Simon spatial interference task (Castel, Balota, Hutchison, Logan, & Yap, 2007;Simon, 1969), and a consonant-vowel/oddeven (CVOE) task-switching task (Huff, Balota, Minear, Aschenbrenner, & Duchek, 2015).…”
Section: Attentional Control Composite Tasksmentioning
confidence: 99%