2013
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.11.011
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Dissociating temporal preparation processes as a function of the inter-trial interval duration

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
66
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 59 publications
(70 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
4
66
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For instance, Vallesi et al (2013) used the variable-FP paradigm with a uniform FP-distribution, and varied the intertrial interval. They observed that an increase of the intertrial interval attenuated the sequential effect and its asymmetry while leaving the average RT–FP function relatively unchanged.…”
Section: Two Explanatory Constructsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…For instance, Vallesi et al (2013) used the variable-FP paradigm with a uniform FP-distribution, and varied the intertrial interval. They observed that an increase of the intertrial interval attenuated the sequential effect and its asymmetry while leaving the average RT–FP function relatively unchanged.…”
Section: Two Explanatory Constructsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, any factor that selectively influences either the recency or the relative frequency of distinctive memory traces will selectively modify the short-term and long-term effects, respectively. For instance, the finding of Vallesi et al (2013) that the duration of the intertrial interval attenuates the asymmetric sequential effect while leaving the average RT–FP function largely unaffected, would reflect that this variable influences the trace weights of recently formed memory traces while leaving the relative frequency of distinctive traces unaffected.…”
Section: Accounting For the Family Of Fp Effectsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…It has been argued that the changing hazard is used by a controlled process that allows participants to strategically apply knowledge about the hazard function to enhance their preparatory state (e.g., Coull et al, 2011;Vallesi et al, 2013;Vallesi, McIntosh, Shallice, & Stuss, 2009;Vallesi & Shallice, 2007). This view requires that participants have access to the changing hazard as time elapses during the foreperiod, but no one has ever specified how people acquire knowledge of the hazard function and how they are able to subsequently apply it.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Most inquiries have started from the idea that the increase of temporal preparation is closely related to the hazard function (e.g., Nobre, Correa, & Coull, 2007;Vallesi, Lozano, & Correa, 2013), which describes the increasing conditional probability of S2 occurrence over time, given that it has not occurred yet (e.g., Luce, 1986). In particular, there exists a close reciprocal relationship between current hazard and mean RT across a variety of foreperiod distributions (Coull, 2009;Cui, Stetson, Montague, & Eagleman, 2009;Janssen & Shadlen, 2005;Näätänen, 1971;Trillenberg et al, 2000), which has led to the widespread belief that hazard drives temporal preparation or temporal expectancy (e.g., Coull, Cheng, & Meck, 2011;Niemi & Näätänen, 1981;Stuss et al, 2005;Vallesi et al, 2013;Vallesi & Shallice, 2007).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%