1999
DOI: 10.3758/bf03210816
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Dissociated backward priming effects in lexical decision and pronunciation tasks

Abstract: Backward priming was examined at 150-and 500-msec prime-target stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs) using visually presented primes and targets in lexical decision and pronunciation tasks. Two kinds of backward relations were used: compound items for which targets and primes formed a word in the backward direction (e.g., prime: HOP; target: bell), and noncompound items for which targets and primes did not form a word but were associatively related in the backward but not the forward direction (e.g., prime: BABY;… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

5
56
5

Year Published

2002
2002
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 53 publications
(66 citation statements)
references
References 31 publications
5
56
5
Order By: Relevance
“…Contrary to episodic retrieval, retrospective semantic matching is most often thought to reflect a strategic process with backward priming effects often used as markers of such a strategy (Kahan et al, 1999;Neely, 1991;Peterson & Simpson, 1989;Seidenberg, Waters, Sanders, & Langer, 1984;Shelton & Martin, 1992). The absence of backward priming in the present experiment therefore suggests that the participants were not engaged in this process.…”
Section: Episodic Retrieval and Semantic Matchingmentioning
confidence: 34%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…Contrary to episodic retrieval, retrospective semantic matching is most often thought to reflect a strategic process with backward priming effects often used as markers of such a strategy (Kahan et al, 1999;Neely, 1991;Peterson & Simpson, 1989;Seidenberg, Waters, Sanders, & Langer, 1984;Shelton & Martin, 1992). The absence of backward priming in the present experiment therefore suggests that the participants were not engaged in this process.…”
Section: Episodic Retrieval and Semantic Matchingmentioning
confidence: 34%
“…The data are reported from 40 critical asymmetrically associated noncompound word pairs (e.g., dentist-pain) taken from Kahan et al (1999), Peterson and Simpson (1989), and Thompson-Schill, Kurtz, and Gabrieli (1998). The University of South Florida Word Association Norms (Nelson, McEvoy, & Schreiber, 1989) were used to select word pairs from these studies that contained a strong forward association (mean forward association = .26) yet little to no backward association (mean backward association = .01).…”
Section: Stimulimentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Woolsey, 1997), (2) prime or target visibility (Balota, 1983;Balota, Yap, Cortese, & Watson, 2008;Kiefer & Martens, 2010;Stolz & Neely, 1995;Thomas, Neely, & O'Connor, 2012), (3) response tasks (Hutchison, Balota, Cortese, & Watson, 2008;Kahan, Neely, & Forsythe, 1999;Pecher, Zeelenberg, &Raaijmakers, 1998), (4) prime-target relation type (Chiarello, Burgess, Richards, & Pollock, 1990;Hodgson, 1991;Moss, Ostrin, Tyler, & Marslen-Wilson, 1995), and (5) developmental or individual differences (Balota & Duchek, 1988;Moritz, Woodward, Küppers, Lausen, & Schickel, 2003;Plaut & Booth, 2000;Stanovich & West, 1979). Interactions within such factorial designs have provided the foundation for developing theories of semantic organization of knowledge and memory retrieval.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%