2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2016.07.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Dissecting the influence of the collinear and flanking bars in White’s effect

Abstract: In White’s effect equiluminant test patches placed on the black and white bars of a square-wave grating appear different in brightness. The illusion has generated intense interest because the direction of the brightness effect does not correlate with the amount of black or white border in contact with the test patch, or in its general vicinity. Therefore, unlike brightness induction effects such as simultaneous contrast, White’s effect is not consistent with explanations based on contrast or assimilation that … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

1
4
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 38 publications
1
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The targets in white are decrements and their brightness scale similarly crispens near the white collinear bar as well as near the black minimum luminance. This dominant role for the collinear, but not the flanking, contrast in White's (1979) effect has previously been implied (e.g., Betz et al., 2015a ; Betz et al., 2015b ; Blakeslee, Padmanabhan, & McCourt, 2016 ).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 59%
“…The targets in white are decrements and their brightness scale similarly crispens near the white collinear bar as well as near the black minimum luminance. This dominant role for the collinear, but not the flanking, contrast in White's (1979) effect has previously been implied (e.g., Betz et al., 2015a ; Betz et al., 2015b ; Blakeslee, Padmanabhan, & McCourt, 2016 ).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 59%
“…One obvious possibility is that the Himba possess uncorrected refractive errors (although previous tests of visual acuity have not supported this). While White’s illusion—like simultaneous lightness contrast—is purely contrast-based at the low spatial frequencies of grating used here (Blakeslee, Padmanabhan, & McCourt, 2016), refractive errors would effectively cause assimilation effects that also affect White’s illusion (Blakeslee & McCourt, 2004). Assimilation effects, however, enhance White’s illusion, suggesting that refractive differences do not play an important causal role here.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…The oriented filter explanation for the White effect is further supported by a more recent study (Blakeslee et al, 2016 ) in which the luminance of the collinear and flanking bars was independently manipulated in order to investigate their separate influence on target patch matching luminance (apparent intensity or brightness). The inducing grating was a 0.5 c/d square-wave and target patches measured 1.0° in width and either 0.5° or 3.0° in height.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 80%
“…Optical blurring does not obviate the need for a neural explanation of the effect as well, however, since the signature of optical blurring (a linear increase in magnitude as a function of inducing luminance), is not always observed (Jameson and Hurvich, 1989;Hong and Shevell, 2004). In other instances (discussed in detail below), effects that are described as brightness assimilation have been shown to result from strong directional brightness contrast mechanisms (Blakeslee et al, 2016); while others, which are the topic of the present article, appear to be the manifestation of remote brightness contrast mechanisms (Shapley and Reid, 1985;Reid and Shapley, 1988).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%