1981
DOI: 10.1177/027112148100100210
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Discontinuity and instability in early development: implications for assessment

Abstract: T HE TERM development implies a high degree of continuity and stability in behavior change across time. Yet, in general, the preschool years are characterized by instabilities in development and lack of continuity in behavior change. In examining the implications of developmental discontinuities and instabilities for the assessment of infant and preschool-aged children, this article emphasizes the interpretation of assessment-related data. The term assessment will be used broadly to mean measurement activities… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

1982
1982
2009
2009

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 14 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Furthermore, the available data on the relative continuity and stability of performance on intellectual measures during this period is equivocal. A number of studies and conceptual analyses suggest that the second and third years of life represent periods of major discontinuity and reorganization in cognitive or intellectual function (Birns & Golden, 1972; Dunst & Rheingrover, 1981; Lewis, Jaskir, & Enright, 1986; Roe, 1977); several reports in this category present data supporting qualitative transition(s) at or just before the second year of age (Reznick et al, 1997; McCall et al, 1977; Molfese & Acheson, 1997). Other studies, however, report relatively smooth change with significant prediction from standardized measures of intellectual performance during the second and third years to preschool IQ (Aylward, 2006; Humphreys & Davey, 1988; Ramey, Campbell, & Nicholson, 1973; Siegel, 1981; Wilson, 1983).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, the available data on the relative continuity and stability of performance on intellectual measures during this period is equivocal. A number of studies and conceptual analyses suggest that the second and third years of life represent periods of major discontinuity and reorganization in cognitive or intellectual function (Birns & Golden, 1972; Dunst & Rheingrover, 1981; Lewis, Jaskir, & Enright, 1986; Roe, 1977); several reports in this category present data supporting qualitative transition(s) at or just before the second year of age (Reznick et al, 1997; McCall et al, 1977; Molfese & Acheson, 1997). Other studies, however, report relatively smooth change with significant prediction from standardized measures of intellectual performance during the second and third years to preschool IQ (Aylward, 2006; Humphreys & Davey, 1988; Ramey, Campbell, & Nicholson, 1973; Siegel, 1981; Wilson, 1983).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, the screening results for children with significant developmental problems are likely to be more stable over time than those of normal children (Dunst & Rheingrover, 1981;Rubin & Balow, 1980). As compared to normal children, whose functioning often reflects a relatively unpredictable developmental process, the test performance of children with extreme disabilities is less subject to irregular change over time.…”
Section: Issues In the Use Of Classificational And Correlational Methmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…, the diversity of commercial screening instruments (McCarthy, 1989)' and an inconsistent relationship between predictive measures and reading performance (Badian, 1976;Dunst & Rheingrover, 1981). Consequently, unless a systematic evaluation of current screening practices is undertaken, their predictive power for the high-incidence exceptionalities will remain speculative.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%