1988
DOI: 10.1037/0021-843x.97.3.296
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Direct versus indirect suggestions, archaic involvement, and hypnotic experience.

Abstract: This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
9
0

Year Published

1990
1990
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
1
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…A secondary interest of the current study was to assess the extent to which the length of the delay between the first and second screening affects the outcome of the screen and the psychometric properties of the measurement tool. Repeated assessment of suggestibility can negatively affect the suggestibility scores, for instance, if the delay amid the two occasions takes only a few days or weeks (Barber & Calverley, 1966;Fassler et al, 2008;Lynn et al, 1988). This reduction in suggestibility may be caused by boredom; the participants can become disengaged with the procedure by virtue of finding it repetitive (Barber & Calverley, 1966;Fassler et al, 2008).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…A secondary interest of the current study was to assess the extent to which the length of the delay between the first and second screening affects the outcome of the screen and the psychometric properties of the measurement tool. Repeated assessment of suggestibility can negatively affect the suggestibility scores, for instance, if the delay amid the two occasions takes only a few days or weeks (Barber & Calverley, 1966;Fassler et al, 2008;Lynn et al, 1988). This reduction in suggestibility may be caused by boredom; the participants can become disengaged with the procedure by virtue of finding it repetitive (Barber & Calverley, 1966;Fassler et al, 2008).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The question about the stability of hypnotic suggestibility over periods of few days or even decades have inspired various research projects (e.g. Fassler, Lynn, & Knox, 2008;Lynn, Weekes, Matyi, & Neufeld, 1988;Piccione, Hilgard, & Zimbardo, 1989). To assess the stability of hypnotic suggestibility, we recruited half of the sample from the subject pool of the year of 2016 and the other half from the year of 2017, both of whom have already received offline screening.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…style, provide the subject with a range of appropriate responses, and convey the implication that the subject is central to hypnotic events. Three recent studies found that despite responding equivalently in terms of overt response, subjects who received permissive suggestions rated their responses as feeling more voluntary than did subjects who received authoritatively worded suggestions (Lynn, Neufeld, & Matyi, 1987;Lynn, Weekes, Matyi, & Neufeld, 1988).…”
Section: Permissive Versus Authoritative Suggestionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Firstly, in the field of social psychology, suggestibility is still viewed as a group phenomenon that can be manipulated (Gheorghiu, 1988). Secondly, hypnotized subjects' rapport with their hypnotist may be more intense in a group than an individual setting (Lynn, Weekes, Matyi, & Neufeld, 1988). Register and Kihlstrom (1986) found that only 36% of subjects defined as highly hypnotizable using the Harvard Group Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale (HGHSS:A; Shor & Orne, 1962) were so defined using the individually administered Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale (SHSS; Weitzenhoffer & Hilgard, 1962).…”
Section: Hypnosis Group Dynamics and Reality Testingmentioning
confidence: 99%