2012
DOI: 10.1242/jeb.077255
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Dine or dash? Turbulence inhibits blue crab navigation in attractive–aversive odor plumes by altering signal structure encoded by the olfactory pathway

Abstract: SUMMARYBlue crabs can distinguish and navigate to attractive (food) odors even when aversive odors (injured crab metabolites) are released nearby. Blue crabs in these conditions detect the aversive odor and avoid it, but find the attractive source with nearly the same success rate as when the attractive source is presented alone. Spatially and temporally distinct odor filaments appear to signal to foragers that the two odor sources are not co-located, and hence navigating to the attractive odor entails an acce… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

1
29
1

Year Published

2013
2013
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 27 publications
(31 citation statements)
references
References 43 publications
1
29
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Furthermore, statistical interactions between the effects of the two types of plants are not well supported by data; they could have been an indication that one plant maintained a net attractive or repulsive effect despite the presence of another plant with opposite properties (one stimulus "overshadowing the other"; see, e.g., Perez et al 2015). These results are qualitatively similar to those from previous studies on odour mixtures, in particular mixtures involving both attractive and aversive stimuli (Tomba et al 2001;Weissburg et al 2012;Späthe et al 2013). The absence of a response from individuals exposed to a heterogeneous stimulus (blend of palatable and repulsive plants) can result from two main mechanisms.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 72%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Furthermore, statistical interactions between the effects of the two types of plants are not well supported by data; they could have been an indication that one plant maintained a net attractive or repulsive effect despite the presence of another plant with opposite properties (one stimulus "overshadowing the other"; see, e.g., Perez et al 2015). These results are qualitatively similar to those from previous studies on odour mixtures, in particular mixtures involving both attractive and aversive stimuli (Tomba et al 2001;Weissburg et al 2012;Späthe et al 2013). The absence of a response from individuals exposed to a heterogeneous stimulus (blend of palatable and repulsive plants) can result from two main mechanisms.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 72%
“…Electrophysiological studies directly investigating neuronal responses to olfactory cues might be able to discriminate these two hypotheses (Peschel et al 1996;Cummins and Wyeth 2014). In blue crabs (Callinectes sapidus M.J. Rathbun, 1896), experiments show that the absence of response to conflicting cues results from the active cancellation of the foraging response (Weissburg et al 2012); a similar mechanism in C. aspersum would tend to support the second hypothesis (i.e., change in response bias).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Certain cues, such as correlations between the flow kinematics and odorant concentration that the animal sense through the chemoreceptors and mechanoreceptors, can provide valuable information regarding the plume source. However, due to the high intermittency and temporal and spatial variability of the plume, this often reduces the ability of organisms, such as the blue crab, Callinectes sapidus , to successfully navigate to the source of an attractive odor [16]. …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In particular, odorants that arise from a single source would arrive at the olfactory organ synchronously, whereas odorants that arise from multiple sources would differ in their arrival times (Erskine et al, 2019;Hopfield, 1991). Accordingly, invertebrates can use both spatial and temporal information from odor plumes for odor-background segregation (spatial: (Andersson et al, 2011;Baker et al, 1998;Hopfield and Gelperin, 1989;Weissburg et al, 2012); temporal: (Saha et al, 2013;Sehdev et al, 2019;Szyszka et al, 2012). Remarkably, tobacco hawk moths can segregate odorant sources separated by only 1 millimeter (Baker et al, 1998), and honey bees can use odorant onset asynchronies as short as 6 milliseconds to segregate a known target odorant (odorant with innate or learned valence) from a background odorant (Szyszka et al, 2012).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%