2003
DOI: 10.3758/bf03196506
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Differential effects of object orientation on imaginary object /viewer transformations

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2005
2005
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
4

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Instead of imagining themselves in the position of the pictured body, observers would solve same-different tasks by using an "object-based spatial transformation" or object mental rotation to align both the reference and comparison postures. However, although viewer mental rotation is generally at an advantage over object mental rotation (Wraga, Creem, & Proffitt, 1999), this is not the case when imagining physically impossible self-rotations such as a roll motion in the coronal plane (M. Carpenter & Proffitt, 2001;Creem, Wraga, & Proffitt, 2001;Van Lier, 2003), corresponding to the picture-plane rotations investigated by Zacks and colleagues (2001Zacks and colleagues ( , 2002Zacks and colleagues ( , 2003. The latter findings certainly run counter the hypotheses of Zacks and colleagues.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Instead of imagining themselves in the position of the pictured body, observers would solve same-different tasks by using an "object-based spatial transformation" or object mental rotation to align both the reference and comparison postures. However, although viewer mental rotation is generally at an advantage over object mental rotation (Wraga, Creem, & Proffitt, 1999), this is not the case when imagining physically impossible self-rotations such as a roll motion in the coronal plane (M. Carpenter & Proffitt, 2001;Creem, Wraga, & Proffitt, 2001;Van Lier, 2003), corresponding to the picture-plane rotations investigated by Zacks and colleagues (2001Zacks and colleagues ( , 2002Zacks and colleagues ( , 2003. The latter findings certainly run counter the hypotheses of Zacks and colleagues.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, although viewer mental rotation is generally at an advantage over object mental rotation (Wraga, Creem, & Proffitt, 1999), this is not the case when imagining physically impossible self-rotations such as a roll motion in the coronal plane (M. Carpenter & Proffitt, 2001; Creem, Wraga, & Proffitt, 2001; Van Lier, 2003), corresponding to the picture-plane rotations investigated by Zacks and colleagues (2001, 2002, 2003). The latter findings certainly run counter the hypotheses of Zacks and colleagues.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, when they were asked the location of a particular item or asked to construct a copy of the transformed array, array rotations were easier. Array rotations also have been found to be easier when the stimuli were simple symmetric line drawings (van Lier, 2003).…”
Section: Behavioral Datamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Phenomenologically, the task-frame in this condition remained static 6 The interpretation of a linear relationship between response time and displacement angle as reflecting physical rotations has been criticised on grounds that many mental imagery tasks are found to be cognitively penetrable [205], and that mental rotation is a visually complex task ( [25; 98]; but see [233]). Regardless of the actual matching process underlying the mental process however, mental rotation for static displays is thought to be based on object orientation, because the viewpoint of the observer always remains the same [266].…”
Section: Testing the Frame Co-registration Cost Hypothesismentioning
confidence: 99%