2010
DOI: 10.1177/0142723710379957
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Dialogical factors in toddlers’ use of clitic pronouns

Abstract: Young (1;9—2;4) children’s use of third person clitic subject pronouns in natural dialogues was examined in both longitudinal and cross-sectional data. Considering that young children mainly use pronouns in the context of referential continuity, this study aims at identifying some of the factors that affect this use. Two possible dialogical factors are examined: (1) the use of clitic pronouns can be interpreted as a reproduction of the adult’s discourse, either by taking up whole utterances containing a pronou… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
9
0
3

Year Published

2012
2012
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 27 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 31 publications
1
9
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Even before they have a fully developed Theory of Mind, three-year-olds are already at least partly sensitive to the same constraints that regulate referential choice in adult speakers (see Allen, Hughes & Skarabela, 2015, for a review). Pre-school children are more likely to omit arguments, or use reduced expressions, when they are part of the common ground either through joint attention (Skarabela, 2007), previous linguistic mention (Allen & Schröder, 2003; Clancy, 2003; Guerriero, Oshima-Takane & Kuriyama, 2006; Stephens, 2015), or prior mention and/or perceptual availability (Campbell, Brooks & Tomasello, 2000; De Cat, 2011; Matthews, Lieven, Theakston & Tomasello, 2006; Rozendaal & Baker, 2010; Salazar Orvig, Marcos, Morgensterns, Hassan, Leber-Marin & Parès, 2010a; Salazar Orvig, Marcos, Morgensterns, Hassan, Leber-Marin & Parès, 2010b).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Even before they have a fully developed Theory of Mind, three-year-olds are already at least partly sensitive to the same constraints that regulate referential choice in adult speakers (see Allen, Hughes & Skarabela, 2015, for a review). Pre-school children are more likely to omit arguments, or use reduced expressions, when they are part of the common ground either through joint attention (Skarabela, 2007), previous linguistic mention (Allen & Schröder, 2003; Clancy, 2003; Guerriero, Oshima-Takane & Kuriyama, 2006; Stephens, 2015), or prior mention and/or perceptual availability (Campbell, Brooks & Tomasello, 2000; De Cat, 2011; Matthews, Lieven, Theakston & Tomasello, 2006; Rozendaal & Baker, 2010; Salazar Orvig, Marcos, Morgensterns, Hassan, Leber-Marin & Parès, 2010a; Salazar Orvig, Marcos, Morgensterns, Hassan, Leber-Marin & Parès, 2010b).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…), uses of demonstrative ça (often used for non-focused elements introduced into the conversation), or lexical noun phrases (e.g. Salazar Orvig, Marcos, Morgenstern, Hassan, Leber-Marin & Parès, 2010). Indeed, patterns of adult verb construction uses in conversation with children may account better for children's first uses than simple frequency of verbs in adult speech (Veneziano & Parisse, 2010).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…All participants were native Hebrew speakers from medium-high SES, with no language disorders or other developmental problems. Triadic conversation was selected for this study as requiring complex turn-taking, the usage of pronominal reference, and the comprehension of the role of a third person in conversation (Salazar Orvig et al 2010). Each of the 18 triadic sessions was conducted in one of the children's homes, audio-recorded and transcribed.…”
Section: The Corpusmentioning
confidence: 99%