2013
DOI: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.287
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Developmental Twin Study of Attention Problems

Abstract: The genetic and environmental link between attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in childhood and the adult manifestation of the disorder is poorly understood because of a lack of longitudinal studies with crossinformant data. Objective: To explore the relative contribution of genetic and environmental influences on symptoms of attention problems from childhood to early adulthood. Design: Analysis was conducted using longitudinal structural equation modeling with multiple informants. Setting: The Swedish Tw… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

9
65
0
2

Year Published

2013
2013
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 151 publications
(76 citation statements)
references
References 52 publications
(71 reference statements)
9
65
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…The finding is in line with a recent cross-informant twin study reporting that the heritability of ADHD in 19–20 year olds was 78%, when both self and parent ratings were combined into a composite index of ADHD symptoms to adjust for rater bias (Chang et al, 2013) and also with prior family studies suggesting a high familial loading on ADHD in adults (Biederman et al, 1996, Biederman et al, 1995, Faraone et al, 2000, Faraone, 2004), but is inconsistent with what has been reported in twin studies of self-rated ADHD in adults (Reiersen et al, 2008, Boomsma et al, 2010, van den Berg et al, 2006, Larsson et al, 2013a). One increasingly recognized explanation to previous reports of low heritability for ADHD in adults is increased contribution of measurement error (reflecting accuracy of the measures) associated with the use of self-ratings (Kan et al, 2013, Franke et al, 2012, Merwood et al, 2013, Chang et al, 2013). Another explanation is that the clinical diagnosis of ADHD in adults reflects persistence of the childhood disorder (i.e., childhood onset), whereas cross-sectional self-ratings may also reflect adult-onset ADHD-like symptoms (i.e., phenocopies) involving different genetic and environmental processes.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 87%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The finding is in line with a recent cross-informant twin study reporting that the heritability of ADHD in 19–20 year olds was 78%, when both self and parent ratings were combined into a composite index of ADHD symptoms to adjust for rater bias (Chang et al, 2013) and also with prior family studies suggesting a high familial loading on ADHD in adults (Biederman et al, 1996, Biederman et al, 1995, Faraone et al, 2000, Faraone, 2004), but is inconsistent with what has been reported in twin studies of self-rated ADHD in adults (Reiersen et al, 2008, Boomsma et al, 2010, van den Berg et al, 2006, Larsson et al, 2013a). One increasingly recognized explanation to previous reports of low heritability for ADHD in adults is increased contribution of measurement error (reflecting accuracy of the measures) associated with the use of self-ratings (Kan et al, 2013, Franke et al, 2012, Merwood et al, 2013, Chang et al, 2013). Another explanation is that the clinical diagnosis of ADHD in adults reflects persistence of the childhood disorder (i.e., childhood onset), whereas cross-sectional self-ratings may also reflect adult-onset ADHD-like symptoms (i.e., phenocopies) involving different genetic and environmental processes.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 87%
“…This is because twin studies on ADHD in adults have used self-ratings, while studies on children have used other informants (i.e., parent and teacher ratings). The few studies on this topic suggest that the heritability of self-rated ADHD is low in both adults and adolescents (Kan et al, 2013, Merwood et al, 2013), and that the heritability of ADHD in adults is substantial when both self and parent ratings are combined into a composite index of ADHD (Chang et al, 2013). Together, these findings suggest that the low heritability for ADHD in adults may be best explained by rater effects.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Finally, the exclusive reliance on parent report as outcome measure in adolescence and young adulthood is also a potential limitation. However, research to date suggests the use of parent ratings of ADHD symptoms as the most reliable source of information compared to self-ratings, in both children and adults (Chang et al , 2013, Epstein et al , 2000, Merwood et al , 2013). Furthermore, we wished to use the same rater for both baseline and outcomes measures as otherwise any observed changes might related to change in the rater (parent to self-report) rather than reflecting the developmental change.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The intergenerational continuity of ADHD (i.e., parental and offspring ADHD) reflects substantial shared genetic influences: In a meta-analysis of 22 studies, genetic factors accounted for 71% and 73% of the variance in inattention and hyperactivity symptoms, respectively (Nikolas & Burt, 2010). However, imperfect concordance rates of ADHD among monozygotic (MZ) twins indicate the importance of environment influences: Among 1,480 MZ and dizygotic twins, the cross-MZ twin correlations at age 8, 13, 16, and 19 ranged from r =.53 to .70 for parent-rated attention problems and from r =.09 to .33 for self-rated attention problems (Chang, Lichtenstein, Asherson, & Larsson, 2013). Furthermore, only 25 to 50% of parents with ADHD have a biological child with ADHD (Biederman, Faraone, & Monuteaux, 2002; Chronis et al, 2003; Wilens et al, 2005).…”
Section: Intergenerational Continuity Of Adhdmentioning
confidence: 99%