2010
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.05.010
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Developmental time course of the acquisition of sequential egocentric and allocentric navigation strategies

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

15
114
1

Year Published

2013
2013
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 111 publications
(140 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
15
114
1
Order By: Relevance
“…They found that both frames of reference were present in 5-year-old children (see also Newcombe & Huttenlocher, 2003). Bullens, Iglói, Berthoz, Postma, and Rondi-Reig (2010) tested spatial navigation abilities of 5-, 7-, and 10-year-old children by means of the StarMaze paradigm (Iglói, Zaoui, Berthoz, & Rondi-Reig, 2009). Results showed a gradual development from an egocentric to a more stable map-like representation between 5 and 10 years of age.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…They found that both frames of reference were present in 5-year-old children (see also Newcombe & Huttenlocher, 2003). Bullens, Iglói, Berthoz, Postma, and Rondi-Reig (2010) tested spatial navigation abilities of 5-, 7-, and 10-year-old children by means of the StarMaze paradigm (Iglói, Zaoui, Berthoz, & Rondi-Reig, 2009). Results showed a gradual development from an egocentric to a more stable map-like representation between 5 and 10 years of age.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While spatial strategies employed by adults are based on the parallel cooperation of egocentric and allocentric frames of reference (e.g., Nadel & Hardt, 2004), the capacity of children to efficiently integrate them seems to emerge at around 6 years of age (Bullens et al, 2010;Nardini et al, 2006; see also Nardini, Jones, Bedford, & Braddick, 2008). Some authors have suggested that this period is crucial for the development of executive functions that enable the acquisition of new skills such as selecting the appropriate spatial strategy and combining spatial and non-spatial environmental properties (Hermer & Spelke, 1994;Nardini et al, 2008Nardini et al, , 2009Vasilyeva & Lourenco, 2012).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…23 It has been demonstrated that children up to the age of 10 fail to use an allocentric reference frame when retrieving previously learnt paths in a radial maze. 24 This failure, rather than to a pure lack of allocentric spatial encoding, has been attributed to a difficulty in switching from egoto allocentric encoding. This problem has been also addressed by Poirel et al 25 on a Piagetian task of length conservation (to judge which line is longer between a zigzag one and a straight counterpart), performed in either an egocentric perspective (ground view) or an allocentric one (aerial view), showing that allocentric errors decreased between 7 years and 10 years of age.…”
Section: The Typical and Atypical Development Of Navigationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These path-sparing errors seem to have a different meaning in CP than in typical development; in fact, their frequency is negatively related to span, signalling that here they represent a maladaptive sequential strategy. This could be due to the hyperactivation of left-brain spatial processes, such as lefthippocampal sequential egocentric memory (learning a sequence of movements), 24 without sufficient contribution from the contralateral areas subserving the simultaneous, non-serial representation of spatial relationships. 30 …”
Section: The Typical and Atypical Development Of Navigationmentioning
confidence: 99%