2001
DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.209
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Developmental origin of the animate–inanimate distinction.

Abstract: The authors examine recent theoretical perspectives of the development of the animate-inanimate distinction in infancy. From these theoretical views emerge 7 characteristic properties, each related to physical or psychological causality, that distinguish animates from inanimates. The literature is reviewed for evidence of infants' ability to perceive and understand each of these properties. Infants associate some animate properties with people by 6 months, but they do not associate the appropriate properties t… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

15
215
0
1

Year Published

2003
2003
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 362 publications
(247 citation statements)
references
References 143 publications
15
215
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Moreover, we need to consider the role that specific patterns of motion might play in the formation of these representations. We know that moving objects are generally more salient than static objects, and that some aspects of motion, such as being self-starting and having an irregular path, may be of particular importance for infants' categorisation of animate versus inanimate objects (Opfer, 2002;Rakison & Poulin-Dubois, 2001). However, there has been little research examining how more specific aspects of so-called biological motion might allow infants to distinguish between humans and animals.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, we need to consider the role that specific patterns of motion might play in the formation of these representations. We know that moving objects are generally more salient than static objects, and that some aspects of motion, such as being self-starting and having an irregular path, may be of particular importance for infants' categorisation of animate versus inanimate objects (Opfer, 2002;Rakison & Poulin-Dubois, 2001). However, there has been little research examining how more specific aspects of so-called biological motion might allow infants to distinguish between humans and animals.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Perhaps the least ambiguous of all the motion characteristics displayed by different object kinds is that of self-propulsion or onset of motion, because only animals and people tend to move without some external physical cause (Premack, 1990;Rakison & Poulin-Dubois, 2001). Despite the potential importance of this cue in delineating different object kinds, there is a dearth of empirical evidence concerning when and how infants learn which things in the world are self-propelled and which are not.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This allowed us to examine in greater detail the extent to which the activation of the sensorimotor system might be specific for humans being touched versus objects being touched. Based on findings that infants have different action expectations concerning humans and nonliving objects (e.g., Rakison & Poulin-Dubois, 2001;Woodward, 1998), we expected a similar differentiation in their sensorimotor system between a human hand versus an object being touched/not-touched.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Behavioural studies already support the view that infants can distinguish between animate and inanimate agents (e.g., Woodward, 1998). Yet, the psychological basis is theoretically debated (e.g., Legerstee, 1992;Rakison & Poulin-Dubois, 2001). The present study contributes to this debate by further clarifying the neurocognitive processes underlying this distinction.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation