2000
DOI: 10.1006/brln.1999.2166
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Developmental Aspects of Verbal Fluency and Confrontation Naming in Children

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

27
184
1
13

Year Published

2005
2005
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 214 publications
(231 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
27
184
1
13
Order By: Relevance
“…Their level of performance on this animal fluency task is not lower than what has been reported in primarily white children from middle class backgrounds in the United States (Halperin et al, 1989) or Canadian, English-speaking children (Regard et al, 1982). Also, the present normative findings are compatible with norms obtained from Italian children studied in the province of Milan (Riva et al, 2000). It should be noted that despite their minority status and socioeconomic background, all children in the present investigation were not requiring special education services and their estimated intellectual ability was within the average range.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 83%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Their level of performance on this animal fluency task is not lower than what has been reported in primarily white children from middle class backgrounds in the United States (Halperin et al, 1989) or Canadian, English-speaking children (Regard et al, 1982). Also, the present normative findings are compatible with norms obtained from Italian children studied in the province of Milan (Riva et al, 2000). It should be noted that despite their minority status and socioeconomic background, all children in the present investigation were not requiring special education services and their estimated intellectual ability was within the average range.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 83%
“…Effect sizes for age (which covaries with grade level) typically account for about 25% of the variance with correlations being in the range of 1.50 for school-age children, ages 6 through 14 (Matute et al, 2004). Differences in rapid generation of animal names between boys and girls, during the school-age years, are often not found (Ardila & Rosselli, 1994;Halperin et al, 1989;Riva et al, 2000) and if they are reported, gender differences are minimal (Regard et al, 1982).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Semantic fluency involves strategic scheduling and monitoring [22]. Performance increases linearly in children between the ages of 5 and 12 years [23] and small, but robust decrements were found in elderly compared with young adults [24].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…1) The intellectual profile was evaluated using the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of IntelligenceThird Edition (WPPSI-III), 25 the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition (WISC-III), 26 and the GMDS-R. 2) We used the Language Assessment Test for children ages 3 -6 years and the Boston Naming Test 28 and Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test 10 for children > 6 years to evaluate lexical naming and comprehension, respectively. Verbal fluency was used for semantic and phonological access.…”
Section: Children > 3 Years Of Agementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Verbal fluency was used for semantic and phonological access. 28 3) The mean of the 3 subtests (Digit Span, Coding, and Arithmetic) of the Wechsler scale that were administered was calculated for the evaluation of global attention, whereas the Bell Cancellation Test 6 and the Selective Attention component from the Leiter-R were used to evaluate visual attention. 4) Memory functions were examined using the Test of Memory and Learning (TOMAL).…”
Section: Children > 3 Years Of Agementioning
confidence: 99%