2017
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.05.045
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Development of the error-monitoring system from ages 9–35: Unique insight provided by MRI-constrained source localization of EEG

Abstract: The ability to self-detect errors and dynamically adapt behavior is a cornerstone of higher-level cognition, requiring coordinated activity from a network of neural regions. However, disagreement exists over how the error-monitoring system develops throughout adolescence and early adulthood. The present report leveraged MRI-constrained EEG source localization to detail typical development of the error-monitoring system in a sample of 9–35 year-olds (n=43). Participants performed a flanker task while high-densi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

9
67
2

Year Published

2017
2017
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

3
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 77 publications
(78 citation statements)
references
References 87 publications
9
67
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Specifically, we demonstrate that early-life BI temperament predicts later hypersensitivity toward errors while under social scrutiny, particularly for females (see Figure S6, available online). Moreover, we found no direct relation between hypersensitivity toward errors and social anxiety in adolescence, which could reflect immaturity in the performance-monitoring system 11 as a direct relation has been found in adults. 16 Finally, direct relations with adolescent social anxiety were found for error preoccupation, and the patterns in other portions of the data extended prior models of social anxiety and BI, 17,18 with BI predicting error hypersensitivity, and in turn, error preoccupation correlating with concurrent social anxiety symptoms.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 72%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Specifically, we demonstrate that early-life BI temperament predicts later hypersensitivity toward errors while under social scrutiny, particularly for females (see Figure S6, available online). Moreover, we found no direct relation between hypersensitivity toward errors and social anxiety in adolescence, which could reflect immaturity in the performance-monitoring system 11 as a direct relation has been found in adults. 16 Finally, direct relations with adolescent social anxiety were found for error preoccupation, and the patterns in other portions of the data extended prior models of social anxiety and BI, 17,18 with BI predicting error hypersensitivity, and in turn, error preoccupation correlating with concurrent social anxiety symptoms.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 72%
“…7,8 This measure of performance monitoring 9 is sensitive to error salience, 10 consistent with the notion that BI children display hypersensitivity toward errors. Risk for anxiety among children with BI is increased for those with a relatively large (more negative) ERN, 7,8 possibly reflecting functioning in a salience network 11 encompassing the cingulate, 12 insula, and orbitofrontal cortex 13 —regions also associated with risk for anxiety. 14 However, although the ERN elicited by standard laboratory tasks is useful for identifying BI children at heightened risk for anxiety, this moderation provides incomplete information on mechanisms underlying the link between BI and social anxiety.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In line with this possibility, prior work suggests there is a degree of normative developmental increase in the ERN (Buzzell et al, 2017). Theoretically, increased ERN in adolescence is thought to reflect the transition from sensitivity to external fear to greater internal saliency of threats related to ones’ behavior (Meyer, 2017).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 70%
“…The ERN was initially localized primarily to the anterior cingulate cortex (van Veen & Carter, 2002), a region of the brain where information about pain, threat, and punishment is integrated to change behavior (Shackman et al, 2011). Additionally, more recent work suggests that activity within the posterior cingulate cortex (Buzzell et al, 2017), superior/medial prefrontal cortex (Hochman, Eviatar, Breznitz, Nevat, & Shaul, 2009; Holmes & Pizzagalli, 2008), and insular cortex (Czobor et al, 2017), which operate within a fronto-limbic network involved in emotional salience and attention, also provide a substantial contribution to the scalp-recorded ERN. An enhanced ERN is postulated to be involved in conflict monitoring (Yeung, Botvinick, & Cohen, 2004), increased sensitivity to endogenous threat (Weinberg, Meyer, et al, 2016), and reinforcement learning (Holroyd & Coles, 2002).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%