2015
DOI: 10.1080/23273798.2015.1053814
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Development of multimodal discourse comprehension: cohesive use of space by gestures

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

1
12
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

5
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 14 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 31 publications
1
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This, in turn, helped children find and encode actor‐specific features, which led to better actor recognition memory. It is well‐documented that pointing gestures (e.g., Langton, O'Malley, & Bruce, ) and the deictic component of iconic gestures (i.e., location in gesture space, at which iconic gestures are produced; Sekine & Kita, ) can direct the recipient's attention to particular areas of the interactional space. The current study demonstrates for the first time that iconicity in iconic gesture can also direct the recipient's attention to a particular part of an event that includes the referent of the gesture.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This, in turn, helped children find and encode actor‐specific features, which led to better actor recognition memory. It is well‐documented that pointing gestures (e.g., Langton, O'Malley, & Bruce, ) and the deictic component of iconic gestures (i.e., location in gesture space, at which iconic gestures are produced; Sekine & Kita, ) can direct the recipient's attention to particular areas of the interactional space. The current study demonstrates for the first time that iconicity in iconic gesture can also direct the recipient's attention to a particular part of an event that includes the referent of the gesture.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Behaviorally, they start to comprehend iconic gestures representing entities at around 3 years of age ( Stanfield et al, 2014 ) and iconic gestures representing events at around 4 years of age ( Glasser et al, 2018 ). Studies showed that 3-year olds could not integrate speech and gesture, whereas 5-year old and adults did (e.g., Sekine and Kita, 2015 ; Sekine et al, 2015 ). Moreover, children starting from 6 years of age integrate speech and gesture in an online fashion comparable to adults ( Dick et al, 2012 ; Sekine et al, in press ).…”
Section: Individual Differences In Gesture Processingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previous studies have attributed their results to both dimensions. For instance, while Cassell et al (1999) attribute the effect in their study to handedness, suggesting that addressees associate each hand with a different referent, other studies assume location in space to be the determining factor (e.g., Goodrich Smith and Hudson Kam, 2012; Gunter et al, 2015; Sekine and Kita, 2015, 2017; Gunter and Weinbrenner, 2017). However, none of the studies provide decisive evidence either way.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Some studies suggest that localizing gestures that are spatially congruent with previous gestures can facilitate processing in comparison to spatially incongruent localizing gestures (Cassell et al, 1999), while others suggest that congruent localizing gestures facilitate processing in comparison to speech alone (Gunter et al, 2015; Gunter and Weinbrenner, 2017). Finally, anaphoric localizing gestures are shown to reinforce expectations about referent resolution in speech (Goodrich Smith and Hudson Kam, 2012; Nappa and Arnold, 2014), and to help identify referents (Sekine and Kita, 2015).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation