2002
DOI: 10.1016/s0149-2063(01)00128-3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Determinants of Risk Taking in a Dynamic Uncertain Context

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
18
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
18
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Individuals tend to be risk-averse when choosing between a certain and uncertain gain, and risk-prone when choosing between a certain and uncertain loss (e.g., Kahneman & Tversky, 1979); this behavior may be affected by previous outcomes (see Hollenbeck et al, 1994; Marsh & Kacelnik, 2002; Slattery & Ganster, 2002; Thaler & Johnson, 1990), such that small versus large outcomes may differentially affect subsequent choices. In the present study, there were no differences following either C-S and C-L outcomes or U-S and U-L outcomes across different probabilities of food in the static (Figures 2 and 3) and dynamic phases (see Figure 5).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Individuals tend to be risk-averse when choosing between a certain and uncertain gain, and risk-prone when choosing between a certain and uncertain loss (e.g., Kahneman & Tversky, 1979); this behavior may be affected by previous outcomes (see Hollenbeck et al, 1994; Marsh & Kacelnik, 2002; Slattery & Ganster, 2002; Thaler & Johnson, 1990), such that small versus large outcomes may differentially affect subsequent choices. In the present study, there were no differences following either C-S and C-L outcomes or U-S and U-L outcomes across different probabilities of food in the static (Figures 2 and 3) and dynamic phases (see Figure 5).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Therefore, choice was affected by the relative amount of reinforcement that could be earned (or lost). Humans have also been shown to be affected by whether or not they were informed of a previous gain or loss before making a choice between a certain and an uncertain outcome (Thaler & Johnson, 1990; also see Hollenbeck, Ilgen, Phillips, & Hedlund, 1994; Slattery & Ganster, 2002). These results suggest that previous outcomes in a sequential-choice paradigm may very well produce a dynamic framing effect from trial to trial.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The primary finding of this research is that those higher in risk propensity and sensation seeking are more likely to make risky decisions (e.g. Harris, 2001;Horvath and Zuckerman, 1993;Sitkin and Weingart, 1995;Slattery and Ganster, 2002;Wong and Carducci, 1991;Weber, Blais and Betz, 2002;Zuckerman and Kuhlman, 2000). Although the most robust findings have been found for risky choice, a limited number of studies have also investigated the impact of risk preference on information processing (McDougal, 1995;Taylor and Dunnette, 1974).…”
Section: Risk-taking Propensity/preference and Sensation Seekingmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…For example, evidence suggests the possibility of stable cross-situational risk preferences as a personality trait (Weber and Milliman, 1997) and that these dispositional measures explained at least some of the variance in individual choices between risky courses of action (Slattery and Ganster 2002). Specific risk tolerance is described as a dispositional tendency to take or avoid risks in a specific situation, which is different from general risk tolerance where people exhibit stable preferences over a variety of situations.…”
Section: Consumer Tolerance Of Food Riskmentioning
confidence: 99%