2015
DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhv115
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Deprivation and Recovery of Sleep in Succession Enhances Reflexive Motor Behavior

Abstract: Sleep deprivation impairs inhibitory control over reflexive behavior, and this impairment is commonly assumed to dissipate after recovery sleep. Contrary to this belief, here we show that fast reflexive behaviors, when practiced during sleep deprivation, is consolidated across recovery sleep and, thereby, becomes preserved. As a model for the study of sleep effects on prefrontal cortex-mediated inhibitory control in humans, we examined reflexive saccadic eye movements (express saccades), as well as speeded 2-c… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

1
6
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

3
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 52 publications
1
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…So in order to get a grasp of the exact latency at which the distractor starts to have an impact on the saccade endpoint, we separately analyzed the transition from anticipatory to express saccades in the Close Distractor condition. The transition is expected to take place around 80 ms (e.g., Sprenger et al, 2015;Wenban-Smith & Findlay, 1991). As shown by the previous analysis of the whole latency range, this expectation was corroborated by our data from the first three latency bins.…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 90%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…So in order to get a grasp of the exact latency at which the distractor starts to have an impact on the saccade endpoint, we separately analyzed the transition from anticipatory to express saccades in the Close Distractor condition. The transition is expected to take place around 80 ms (e.g., Sprenger et al, 2015;Wenban-Smith & Findlay, 1991). As shown by the previous analysis of the whole latency range, this expectation was corroborated by our data from the first three latency bins.…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 90%
“…Although most dominantly observed in monkeys, express saccades in humans have also been observed as a peak in the latency distribution that is separate from ''regular'' saccades (Carpenter, 2001;Fischer & Ramsperger, 1986;Reuter-Lorenz, Hughes, & Fendrich, 1991;Weber, Aiple, Fischer, & Latanov, 1992). Most saccades that we make fall outside the latency range of express saccades, but it is estimated that about 10%-20% of all saccades are express saccades (e.g., Amatya et al, 2011;Knox & Wolohan, 2015;Sprenger et al, 2015). The percentage of express saccades increases when there is a temporal gap between the offset of a fixation point and the onset of a visual target (first described by Saslow, 1967).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 81%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Oculomotor behavior, i.e. gaze stability, saccades and smooth pursuit eye movements, was recorded in both groups by a video-based eye tracker (EyeLink II, SR Research, ON, CA) and analysed [14, 15]. A laser target was used as stimulus (size 0.1°) which was projected on a screen 140 cm in front of the participants.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Vigilance task performance was also not correlated with cue detection in the prospective memory task (all p > 0.05), speaking against the possibility that prospective memory performance was affected by general alertness levels. Alternatively, higher overall performance in wake participants might be interpreted in light of recent findings showing that sleep deprivation can be a state of heightened plasticity due to prefrontal disinhibition (Sprenger et al, 2015). In the wake group, disinhibition under sleep deprivation following the first test session might have triggered plastic changes during subsequent recovery sleep, leading to overall higher performance levels at the second test 2 days later.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%