2015
DOI: 10.1111/poms.12343
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Decision Making and Cognition in Multi‐Echelon Supply Chains: An Experimental Study

Abstract: Supply chain performance often depends on the individual decisions of channel members. Even when individuals have access to relevant information, order variation tends to increase when moving up the supply chain, a phenomenon known as the bullwhip effect. While prior research has investigated several structural/environmental factors which can mitigate the bullwhip effect, the underlying behavioral factors contributing to it are an open question. Using a production and distribution decision‐making simulation re… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
87
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 82 publications
(89 citation statements)
references
References 62 publications
(245 reference statements)
2
87
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, using the classical beer game, Croson and Donohue (2006) find evidence of the bullwhip effect in laboratory experiments, even when the demand distribution is shared and all other causes are removed. They attribute this phenomenon to "underweighting the supply line," reported also by Sterman (1989), Niranjan et al (2011), Bloomfield and Kulp (2013), Croson et al (2014), and Narayanan and Moritz (2015), where players may not account for the previous orders that have not yet been received from their supplier ("onorder quantity" in this study) when placing orders in the next period.…”
Section: Bullwhip Effectmentioning
confidence: 83%
“…However, using the classical beer game, Croson and Donohue (2006) find evidence of the bullwhip effect in laboratory experiments, even when the demand distribution is shared and all other causes are removed. They attribute this phenomenon to "underweighting the supply line," reported also by Sterman (1989), Niranjan et al (2011), Bloomfield and Kulp (2013), Croson et al (2014), and Narayanan and Moritz (2015), where players may not account for the previous orders that have not yet been received from their supplier ("onorder quantity" in this study) when placing orders in the next period.…”
Section: Bullwhip Effectmentioning
confidence: 83%
“…This has been investigated in different contexts including the behavior of managers versus students (Bolton, Ockenfels, & Thonemann, ), females versus males (De Véricourt, Jain, Bearden, & Filipowicz, ), and Chinese versus Americans (Cui, Chen, Chen, Gavirneni, & Wang, ). Another class of studies explores the behavioral causes of the bullwhip effect in the supply chain (e.g., Croson & Donohue, ; Croson, Donohue, Katok, & Sterman, ; Narayanan & Moritz, ; Tokar et al., ) and solutions to mitigate the identified behavioral risks (Wu & Katok, ; Tokar, Aloysius, & Waller, ).…”
Section: Literature Classification Based On Operations Contextsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Croson et al (2014) conduct experiments with the Beer Game and suggest coordination risk as an additional behavioral cause. More recently, Narayanan and Moritz () consider the role of subjects’ cognitive profile on the bullwhip effect, while Villa et al . () find that a retailer facing shortages in a competitive environment will amplify its orders to a supplier following an anchoring and adjustment heuristic.…”
Section: Literature Reviewmentioning
confidence: 99%