2011
DOI: 10.1086/661901
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Deciphering Information Encoded in Birdsong: Male Songbirds with Fertile Mates Respond Most Strongly to Complex, Low-Amplitude Songs Used in Courtship

Abstract: JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.. We compared responses of males whose mates were fertile or nonfertile with differences in song structure (SRS vs. LRS and soft LRS), amplitude (SRS and soft LRS vs. LRS), and … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

3
53
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 35 publications
(56 citation statements)
references
References 55 publications
3
53
0
Order By: Relevance
“…visual, tactile, auditory, and olfactory), including low-amplitude songs, can be employed to further court and stimulate the female (Catchpole and Slater 2008). The low-amplitude songs produced during directed courtship can be structurally distinct and more complex than the high-amplitude songs used in mate attraction (Dabelsteen et al 1998, Balsby 2000, Reichard et al 2011. Based on a similar signaling system in crickets, Zuk et al (2008) hypothesized that females may rely predominantly on high-amplitude, mate-attraction songs for information relating to species recognition, and lowamplitude, courtship songs for information about male quality.…”
Section: And Female Preferences Than What Is Currently Recognized (Sementioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…visual, tactile, auditory, and olfactory), including low-amplitude songs, can be employed to further court and stimulate the female (Catchpole and Slater 2008). The low-amplitude songs produced during directed courtship can be structurally distinct and more complex than the high-amplitude songs used in mate attraction (Dabelsteen et al 1998, Balsby 2000, Reichard et al 2011. Based on a similar signaling system in crickets, Zuk et al (2008) hypothesized that females may rely predominantly on high-amplitude, mate-attraction songs for information relating to species recognition, and lowamplitude, courtship songs for information about male quality.…”
Section: And Female Preferences Than What Is Currently Recognized (Sementioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition to high-amplitude signals, many avian and nonavian species are known to produce a variety of lowamplitude signals, which are drastically understudied when compared with their high-amplitude counterparts (Dabelsteen et al 1998, Osiejuk 2011, Reichard et al 2011). Here, we broadly define low-amplitude signals as any acoustic signal produced at a low volume, such that the signal's effective transmission distance is limited to a close-proximity interaction between the sender and receiver.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Traditionally, researchers have focused on high amplitude songs (hereafter broadcast songs) that transmit over a large distance to potentially numerous receivers. Many songbirds also produce low amplitude songs (hereafter soft songs, but also known as short range songs, quiet songs, whispered songs) that are performed at close range during courtship or territorial disputes (Anderson et al, , 2008(Anderson et al, , 2012Dabelsteen, 1990;Dabelsteen and Pedersen, 1988;Dabelsteen et al, 1998;Hof and Hazlett, 2010;Reichard et al, 2011;Titus, 1998). Soft song is a highly aggressive signal in black-throated blue warblers (Dendroica caerulescens) (Hof and Hazlett, 2010), swamp sparrows (Melospiza georgiana) (Ballentine et al, 2008), and song sparrows (Melospiza melodia) .…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…pika, Ochotona princeps: Conner, 1985; tree shrew, Tupaia belangeri: Binz & Zimmermann, 1989; ringtailed lemur, Lemur catta: Macedonia, 1993 Leuchtenberger, Sousa-Lima, Duplaix, Magnusson, & Mourão, 2014), few mammalian studies directly address the potential function and subsequent adaptive benefit of low-amplitude calls. In comparison, a growing body of research on low-amplitude vocalizations in birds reveals that low-amplitude calling plays an important role in warning conspecifics of predator threats (Dabelsteen, McGregor, Lampe, Langmore, & Holland, 1998) and in mediating a multitude of social contexts, such as competitive interactions (Akçay, Tom, Holmes, Campbell, & Beecher, 2011;Searcy & Nowicki, 2006), courtship (Dabelsteen et al, 1998;Reichard, Rice, Vanderbilt, & Ketterson, 2011;Reichard, Rice, Schultz, & Schrock, 2013) and pair bond maintenance (Elie et al, 2010).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%