2012
DOI: 10.1177/1948550612468774
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cultural Differences in Attitudes Toward Action and Inaction

Abstract: The current research examined whether nations differ in their attitudes toward action and inaction. It was anticipated that members of dialectical East Asian societies would show a positive association in their attitudes toward action/inaction. However, members of non-dialectical European-American societies were expected to show a negative association in their attitudes toward action/inaction. Young adults in 19 nations completed measures of dialectical thinking and attitudes toward action/inaction. Results fr… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
16
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 22 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
0
16
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Future anticipated joy mattered, in support of findings for the role of anticipated regret in escalation of commitment ( Wong & Kwong, 2007 ), as did negative feedback, in support of findings regarding the impact of past experiences and outcomes ( Tykocinski & Ortmann, 2011 ; Whyte, 1986 ). People vary in their attitudes and values regarding action-inaction ( Ireland, Hepler, Li, & Albarracín, 2015 ; Zell et al, 2013 ), in their experienced regret over action-inaction ( Gilovich, Wang, Regan, & Nishina, 2003 ), and in tendencies toward change versus status quo maintenance ( Diefendorff, Hall, Lord, & Strean, 2000 ). These differences have been shown to impact escalation decisions, with seemingly conflicting findings.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Future anticipated joy mattered, in support of findings for the role of anticipated regret in escalation of commitment ( Wong & Kwong, 2007 ), as did negative feedback, in support of findings regarding the impact of past experiences and outcomes ( Tykocinski & Ortmann, 2011 ; Whyte, 1986 ). People vary in their attitudes and values regarding action-inaction ( Ireland, Hepler, Li, & Albarracín, 2015 ; Zell et al, 2013 ), in their experienced regret over action-inaction ( Gilovich, Wang, Regan, & Nishina, 2003 ), and in tendencies toward change versus status quo maintenance ( Diefendorff, Hall, Lord, & Strean, 2000 ). These differences have been shown to impact escalation decisions, with seemingly conflicting findings.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Individualism-collectivism was calculated as individualism scores minus collectivism scores to increase ease of interpretation. Multilevel confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) showed measurement equivalence across nations for each scale (all RMSEA < .10, CFI > .99, SRMR < .05; see Zell et al, 2012). …”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Interestingly, omission bias findings showing a preference for inaction over action seem at first to contradict evidence showing general social norms and attitudes favoring action over inaction (Ireland et al, 2015;Zell et al, 2013).…”
Section: Inaction-effectmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…The very definition and acceptability of action may depend on the broader context, with consequences for subsequent cognition, emotions, and behavior. For example, when a society is very action driven (action-inaction country averages: Ireland, Hepler, Li, & Albarracín, 2015;Zell et al, 2013; US states averages: Ireland, Chen, Schwartz, Ungar, & Albarracín, 2016), action may be perceived as insufficient action or even as inaction, given the comparison to a set level of expected action.…”
Section: Inaction-effectmentioning
confidence: 99%