2015
DOI: 10.3758/s13423-015-0959-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cruel to be kind but not cruel for cash: Harm aversion in the dictator game

Abstract: People regularly take prosocial actions, making individual sacrifices for the greater good.Similarly, people generally avoid causing harm to others. These twin desires to do good and avoid harm often align, but can sometimes diverge, creating situations of moral conflict.Here, we examined this moral conflict using a modified dictator game. Participants chose how much money to allocate away from a recipient, who was designated as an orphan, creating a sense of harm. This money was then re-allocated to either th… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
3
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4

Relationship

1
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
1
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Irrespective of the direction of change in risk taking, the differences in choices for self and others are usually attributed to prosocial attitudes, assuming that people choose what they think would be best for the other person. This supposed ubiquitous prosociality conforms with the hyperaltruism observed when people are faced with causing harm to others (Crockett et al, 2014;Perera et al, 2016), but conflicts with findings that people also have a competitive streak, such as when people are happier when their income exceeds that of those around them (e.g., Clark & Oswald, 1996) or even an anti-social streak, such as when they do not contribute to a public good even though it is in their self-interest to do so (Brandts, Saijo & Schram, 2004;Saijo, 2008). This occasional anti-social behavior in social comparison suggests that differences in risk taking for self and other could also be due to anti-social motives.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 68%
“…Irrespective of the direction of change in risk taking, the differences in choices for self and others are usually attributed to prosocial attitudes, assuming that people choose what they think would be best for the other person. This supposed ubiquitous prosociality conforms with the hyperaltruism observed when people are faced with causing harm to others (Crockett et al, 2014;Perera et al, 2016), but conflicts with findings that people also have a competitive streak, such as when people are happier when their income exceeds that of those around them (e.g., Clark & Oswald, 1996) or even an anti-social streak, such as when they do not contribute to a public good even though it is in their self-interest to do so (Brandts, Saijo & Schram, 2004;Saijo, 2008). This occasional anti-social behavior in social comparison suggests that differences in risk taking for self and other could also be due to anti-social motives.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 68%
“… The benefit-harm gained: A measure of how the actors actions will result in a benefit to the receiver or lack thereof (Brañas-Garza et al, 2014 ; Bruner & Kopec, 2018 ; Chiaravutthi, 2019 ; Perera et al, 2016 ). The consideration of wider public benefit and harm (Gillet et al, 2009 ; Lejano & Ingram, 2012 ; Sigmund et al, 2001 ).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The benefit-harm gained: A measure of how the actors actions will result in a benefit to the receiver or lack thereof (Brañas-Garza et al, 2014 ; Bruner & Kopec, 2018 ; Chiaravutthi, 2019 ; Perera et al, 2016 ).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation