2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.01.050
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Corticospinal excitability during imagined and observed dynamic force production tasks: Effortfulness matters

Abstract: Research on motor imagery and action observation has become increasingly important in recent years particularly because of its potential benefits for movement rehabilitation and the optimization of athletic performance (Munzert et al., 2009). Motor execution, motor imagery, and action observation have been shown to rely largely on a similar neural network in motor and motor-related cortical areas (Jeannerod, 2001). Given that motor imagery is a covert stage of an action and its characteristics, it has been ass… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

1
16
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 27 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 55 publications
(81 reference statements)
1
16
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We found no significant difference in ERD KMI between 10% KMI and 40% KMI tasks. There are several studies investigating the association between imagined muscle contraction strength and corticospinal excitability ( Park and Li, 2011 ; Mizuguchi et al, 2013 ; Helm et al, 2015 ). In some of them, significant differences in motor-evoked potential amplitudes during KMI were observed in between low- and high-imagined contraction strength (i.e., 10 vs. 60% of maximal effort), suggesting difference in the corticospinal excitability depending on imagined contraction strength ( Mizuguchi et al, 2013 ; Helm et al, 2015 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We found no significant difference in ERD KMI between 10% KMI and 40% KMI tasks. There are several studies investigating the association between imagined muscle contraction strength and corticospinal excitability ( Park and Li, 2011 ; Mizuguchi et al, 2013 ; Helm et al, 2015 ). In some of them, significant differences in motor-evoked potential amplitudes during KMI were observed in between low- and high-imagined contraction strength (i.e., 10 vs. 60% of maximal effort), suggesting difference in the corticospinal excitability depending on imagined contraction strength ( Mizuguchi et al, 2013 ; Helm et al, 2015 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is important to note, however, that while the majority of evidence supports the effectiveness of MI and AO as independent instruction techniques, there is evidence to the contrary (see Braun et al, 2013; Gatti et al, 2013; Sarasso et al, 2015). Furthermore, it is difficult to draw clear conclusions on the mixed results provided across studies that have compared the potential advantages of motor imagery vs. action observation , both on motor function and neural processes (e.g., Porro et al, 2007; Filimon et al, 2007, 2015; Szameitat et al, 2012; Gatti et al, 2013; Gonzalez-Rosa et al, 2015; Helm et al, 2015). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although motor imagery and action observation rely on a similar action representation, there is a difference between them in aspects of the mechanism of cognitive process. Motor imagery is a knowledge-driven cognitive process that is internally simulated based on information in long-term memory, without an external stimulus (e.g., Murphy, 1994 ; Soohoo et al, 2001 ; Holmes and Calmels, 2008 ; Helm et al, 2015 ). Instead, action observation is a percept-driven cognitive process that is externally guided by an external stimulus, such as a live demonstration or recorded video (e.g., Ram et al, 2007 ; Holmes and Calmels, 2008 ; Vogt et al, 2013 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%