2006
DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.920
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Coordinate transformations in object recognition.

Abstract: A basic problem of visual perception is how human beings recognize objects after spatial transformations. Three central classes of findings have to be accounted for: (a) Recognition performance varies systematically with orientation, size, and position; (b) recognition latencies are sequentially additive, suggesting analogue transformation processes; and (c) orientation and size congruency effects indicate that recognition involves the adjustment of a reference frame. All 3 classes of findings can be explained… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

4
77
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
4
3

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 78 publications
(82 citation statements)
references
References 344 publications
4
77
0
Order By: Relevance
“…On the face of it, this result seems counterintuitive, given that mono stimuli contain a subset of the information present in the stereo condition. Specifically, the key finding in our experiments is that the cue of outline shape seemed to assist performance in Experiments 1 and 2 only in the mono condition; recognition of 180º rotations in the stereo condition of these experiments seemed to be based on a normalization mechanism more closely aligned with the spatial orientation of the object (e.g., Graf, 2006;Tarr, 1995).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…On the face of it, this result seems counterintuitive, given that mono stimuli contain a subset of the information present in the stereo condition. Specifically, the key finding in our experiments is that the cue of outline shape seemed to assist performance in Experiments 1 and 2 only in the mono condition; recognition of 180º rotations in the stereo condition of these experiments seemed to be based on a normalization mechanism more closely aligned with the spatial orientation of the object (e.g., Graf, 2006;Tarr, 1995).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…However, numerous studies have questioned the notion that the cost associated with identifying a rotated object is due to the need to mentally rotate an internal representation of that object (Cheung, Hayward, & Gauthier, 2009;Gauthier et al, 2002;Jolicoeur, Corballis, & Lawson, 1998;Wilson & Farah, 2006). An alternative account of the alignment process views it as a coordinate transformation (Graf, 2006). According to this account, alignment consists of the analog rotation of an internal reference frame to match the stimulus.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, presenting objects in a non-canonical orientation is often used in an attempt to understand constancies in visual object recognition (e.g., Tarr, 1995). There exist numerous demonstrations that performance, across a range of tasks, can be impaired when stimuli are not presented in their canonical orientation (e.g., Corballis, 1988;Graf, 2006;Jolicoeur, 1990a;Kung & Hamm, 2010). This appears to be true for a number of different classes of stimuli and tasks, for example, naming line drawings of natural objects (Jolicoeur, 1985), naming single letters (Jolicoeur & Landau, 1984;Jolicoeur, Snow, & Murray, 1987;Kolers & Perkins, 1969a, b), reading words (Koriat & Norman, 1984), and recognizing scenes (Diwadkar & McNamara, 1997).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Another point that begs discussion is the important role of priming or congruency effects in general object perception (for a review, see Graf 2006). For example, when we look at the moon rising over a distant skyline, it looks much larger than when we see it high in the sky.…”
Section: Experimental Evidencementioning
confidence: 99%