2021
DOI: 10.31234/osf.io/h49u7
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cooperation across multiple game-theoretical paradigms is increased by fear more than anger in selfish individuals

Abstract: Cooperative decisions are well predicted by stable individual differences in social values but it remains unclear how they may be modulated by emotions such as fear and anger. Moving beyond specific decision paradigms, we used a suite of economic games and investigated how experimental inductions of fear or anger affect latent factors of decision making in individuals with selfish or prosocial value orientations. We found that, relative to experimentally induced anger, induced fear elicited higher scores on a … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
1

Relationship

1
0

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 1 publication
(2 citation statements)
references
References 71 publications
(130 reference statements)
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Researchers juxtaposing self-report measures to (economic) decision making paradigms (Maner et al, 2002;Bosman et al, 2005;Ben-Shakhar et al, 2007;Reuben and van Winden, 2008;Hopfensitz and Reuben, 2009;Eimontaite et al, 2013) could use our motivespecific words to better identify the motives underlying specific decisions and investigate whether indeed such motives are related to the corresponding decision patterns anticipated above (Table 1). In fact, recent studies have already adopted a subset of the motive specific words obtained here and found that they can be simultaneously sensitive to corresponding motivational inductions (e.g., of care, power, fear, or anger) and predictive of economic behaviors of interest (Chierchia et al, 2017(Chierchia et al, , 2021Bartke et al, 2019).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 95%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Researchers juxtaposing self-report measures to (economic) decision making paradigms (Maner et al, 2002;Bosman et al, 2005;Ben-Shakhar et al, 2007;Reuben and van Winden, 2008;Hopfensitz and Reuben, 2009;Eimontaite et al, 2013) could use our motivespecific words to better identify the motives underlying specific decisions and investigate whether indeed such motives are related to the corresponding decision patterns anticipated above (Table 1). In fact, recent studies have already adopted a subset of the motive specific words obtained here and found that they can be simultaneously sensitive to corresponding motivational inductions (e.g., of care, power, fear, or anger) and predictive of economic behaviors of interest (Chierchia et al, 2017(Chierchia et al, , 2021Bartke et al, 2019).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…Participants Participants (n = 310, mean age = 27.19, sd = 5.97, females = 163, males = 147) were recruited via email through the Max Planck participant database to take part in two separate studies on economic decision making (Chierchia et al, 2017(Chierchia et al, , 2021. Those studied required a larger sample size than study 1, yet they provided a good opportunity to test these hypotheses FIGURE 1 | A semantic and subjective atlas of seven motives.…”
Section: Study 2 Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%