2013
DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00082
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Control of automated behavior: insights from the discrete sequence production task

Abstract: Work with the discrete sequence production (DSP) task has provided a substantial literature on discrete sequencing skill over the last decades. The purpose of the current article is to provide a comprehensive overview of this literature and of the theoretical progress that it has prompted. We start with a description of the DSP task and the phenomena that are typically observed with it. Then we propose a cognitive model, the dual processor model (DPM), which explains performance of (skilled) discrete key-press… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

14
211
1
1

Year Published

2014
2014
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1

Relationship

3
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 138 publications
(267 citation statements)
references
References 161 publications
(270 reference statements)
14
211
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Research with the DSP task has provided ample support for the idea that motor sequencing skill is based on representations in memory that reduce and eventually even eliminate the reliance on element-specific stimuli (Abrahamse et al, 2013;Rhodes, Bullock, Verwey, Averbeck, & Page, 2004). This idea is based on findings like individual key presses in a familiar keying sequence becoming so fast that stimulusbased selection of individual responses is unlikely.…”
Section: Multiple Sequence Representationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Research with the DSP task has provided ample support for the idea that motor sequencing skill is based on representations in memory that reduce and eventually even eliminate the reliance on element-specific stimuli (Abrahamse et al, 2013;Rhodes, Bullock, Verwey, Averbeck, & Page, 2004). This idea is based on findings like individual key presses in a familiar keying sequence becoming so fast that stimulusbased selection of individual responses is unlikely.…”
Section: Multiple Sequence Representationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This idea is based on findings like individual key presses in a familiar keying sequence becoming so fast that stimulusbased selection of individual responses is unlikely. Execution rate of such familiar sequences appears to decrease only little when element-specific stimuli are no longer displayed, while in contrast, execution rate decreases substantially if only a single element is being altered (Abrahamse et al, 2013;Verwey, 1999Verwey, , 2010. There is general consensus now that skilled motor behavior is based on a practice-, task-, and age-dependent mixture of various sequence representations (Panzer, Gruetzmacher, Ellenbuerger, & Shea, 2014;Shea et al, 2016;Verwey, Shea, & Wright, 2015;Wiestler, Waters-Metenier, & Diedrichsen, 2014).…”
Section: Multiple Sequence Representationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Automatization of component processes precedes the development of hierarchical control (e.g., Abrahamse et al, 2013;Bryan & Harter, 1899;LaBerge & Samuels, 1974;Rhodes et al, 2004;Vallacher & Wegner, 1987). Automaticity develops by strengthening the associations that underlie the skill, and hierarchical processing emerges when the associations become strong enough to support performance without conscious control (LaBerge & Samuels, 1974;Logan, 1988;Schneider & Shiffrin, 1977).…”
Section: The Role Of Automaticity In Hierarchical Control Of Skillmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To implement multiple components in a rapid succession, skilled performance requires hierarchically organized control processes (Lashley, 1951). Although the notion of hierarchical control has appeared in psychological literature many times (e.g., Abrahamse, Ruitenberg, de Kleine, & Verwey, 2013;Bryan & Harter, 1899;Cooper & Shallice, 2000;Leonard & Newell, 1964;MacKay, 1982;Miller, Galanter, & Pribram, 1960;Rhodes, Bullock, Verwey, Averbeck, & Page, 2004;Verwey, 2001), it remains controversial (e.g., Botvinick & Plaut, 2004;Cooper & Shallice, 2006;Elman, 1990). Studies of typewriting are particularly suited to address the hierarchical nature of skilled performance (Fendrick, 1937;Logan & Crump, 2011;Salthouse, 1986;Shaffer, 1975a;Yamaguchi, Crump, & Logan, 2013).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%